Representation and Misrepresentation of Scientific Evidence in Contemporary Tobacco Regulation: A Review of Tobacco Industry Submissions to the UK Government Consultation on Standardised Packaging
Background:
Standardised packaging (SP) of tobacco products is an innovative tobacco control measure opposed by transnational tobacco companies (TTCs) whose responses to the UK government's public consultation on SP argued that evidence was inadequate to support implementing the measure. The government's initial decision, announced 11 months after the consultation closed, was to wait for ‘more evidence’, but four months later a second ‘independent review’ was launched. In view of the centrality of evidence to debates over SP and TTCs' history of denying harms and manufacturing uncertainty about scientific evidence, we analysed their submissions to examine how they used evidence to oppose SP.
Methods and Findings:
We purposively selected and analysed two TTC submissions using a verification-oriented cross-documentary method to ascertain how published studies were used and interpretive analysis with a constructivist grounded theory approach to examine the conceptual significance of TTC critiques. The companies' overall argument was that the SP evidence base was seriously flawed and did not warrant the introduction of SP. However, this argument was underpinned by three complementary techniques that misrepresented the evidence base. First, published studies were repeatedly misquoted, distorting the main messages. Second, ‘mimicked scientific critique’ was used to undermine evidence; this form of critique insisted on methodological perfection, rejected methodological pluralism, adopted a litigation (not scientific) model, and was not rigorous. Third, TTCs engaged in ‘evidential landscaping’, promoting a parallel evidence base to deflect attention from SP and excluding company-held evidence relevant to SP. The study's sample was limited to sub-sections of two out of four submissions, but leaked industry documents suggest at least one other company used a similar approach.
Conclusions:
The TTCs' claim that SP will not lead to public health benefits is largely without foundation. The tools of Better Regulation, particularly stakeholder consultation, provide an opportunity for highly resourced corporations to slow, weaken, or prevent public health policies.
Please see later in the article for the Editors' Summary
Published in the journal:
. PLoS Med 11(3): e32767. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.1001629
Category:
Research Article
doi:
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1001629
Summary
Background:
Standardised packaging (SP) of tobacco products is an innovative tobacco control measure opposed by transnational tobacco companies (TTCs) whose responses to the UK government's public consultation on SP argued that evidence was inadequate to support implementing the measure. The government's initial decision, announced 11 months after the consultation closed, was to wait for ‘more evidence’, but four months later a second ‘independent review’ was launched. In view of the centrality of evidence to debates over SP and TTCs' history of denying harms and manufacturing uncertainty about scientific evidence, we analysed their submissions to examine how they used evidence to oppose SP.
Methods and Findings:
We purposively selected and analysed two TTC submissions using a verification-oriented cross-documentary method to ascertain how published studies were used and interpretive analysis with a constructivist grounded theory approach to examine the conceptual significance of TTC critiques. The companies' overall argument was that the SP evidence base was seriously flawed and did not warrant the introduction of SP. However, this argument was underpinned by three complementary techniques that misrepresented the evidence base. First, published studies were repeatedly misquoted, distorting the main messages. Second, ‘mimicked scientific critique’ was used to undermine evidence; this form of critique insisted on methodological perfection, rejected methodological pluralism, adopted a litigation (not scientific) model, and was not rigorous. Third, TTCs engaged in ‘evidential landscaping’, promoting a parallel evidence base to deflect attention from SP and excluding company-held evidence relevant to SP. The study's sample was limited to sub-sections of two out of four submissions, but leaked industry documents suggest at least one other company used a similar approach.
Conclusions:
The TTCs' claim that SP will not lead to public health benefits is largely without foundation. The tools of Better Regulation, particularly stakeholder consultation, provide an opportunity for highly resourced corporations to slow, weaken, or prevent public health policies.
Please see later in the article for the Editors' Summary
Introduction
Beginning in the mid-1990s, governments globally have sought to transform policymaking and regulatory activity through a number of reforms collectively known in Europe as Better Regulation [1]. Although these reforms are expected to make policymaking more transparent and evidence-based, in some jurisdictions their implementation has been shown to occur under pressure from corporations whose products are damaging to health, including British American Tobacco (BAT), with the expectation that the associated procedures would make it harder to pass public health policies [2]. In the UK, Better Regulation is underpinned by neoliberal assumptions concerning business competitiveness, and official guidance declares that regulation should not ‘impose costs and obligations’ on business and other groups ‘unless a robust and compelling case has been made’ [3]. This process in turn requires the methodical identification and valuation of all potential costs and benefits of proposed regulation, achieved predominantly through impact assessment and stakeholder consultation [4]. Officials are obliged to seek the views of stakeholders, including corporations, on the government's cost and benefit estimates as well as underlying ‘key assumptions and data’ [3]. Commercial entities that will be affected by proposed regulation are thus given an explicit role in evaluating, confirming, or disputing evidence used in formulating those policies.
The requirements of Better Regulation have significant implications for public health, where major corporations with products damaging to health, including transnational tobacco companies (TTCs) and alcohol [5] and food [6],[7] industries, have a long track record (in tobacco's case, dating back to at least the early 1950s) of manipulating and misrepresenting evidence to deny the harms of their products, question the benefits and highlight the costs of public health policies [8]. TTCs have systematically sought to fabricate doubt and controversy over evidence unfavourable to their interests by labelling research demonstrating tobacco's harms as ‘junk science’, commissioning their own research to challenge established evidence on tobacco's harms or policy impacts [9],[10], and promoting a set of industry-specified scientific standards collectively termed ‘sound science’ and ‘good epidemiology practices’ [11],[12]. This evidence of TTC willingness to challenge and undermine the scientific evidence on tobacco harms and on policy impacts [12],[13],[14],[15],[16],[17],[18],[19],[20],[21] raises concerns that stakeholder consultation and impact assessment may make it easier for TTCs to successfully obstruct public health policies and poses a challenge to the underlying assumption of Better Regulation that evidence optimises policymaking, rendering it more rational and efficient.
The UK experience with standardised packaging (SP) of tobacco products provides a major opportunity to explore the tobacco industry's current approach to and use of evidence. SP aims to prevent the use of packaging as a powerful marketing tool by removing all brand imagery and text (other than name) and instead implementing standard shape/colour packs with prominent pictorial health warnings. First mooted in 1986, SP was consistently opposed by the tobacco industry [22] and it was only in December 2012 that Australia became the first country to adopt the policy [23], despite threatened and ongoing litigation by TTCs [24],[25]. The UK was the next country to consider SP formally; following a four-month public consultation and 11 months of deliberation, the government announced that it had decided to ‘wait and see’ how the Australian experience ‘panned out’ before making a decision [26]. Parliamentary debates [26] and media statements indicated that doubts over the adequacy of the evidence (and the related anticipation of legal challenge by TTCs) were the main reason for the government's hesitance [27],[28]. However, four months later, when it became clear that SP might be introduced through House of Lords amendments to unrelated legislation on children and families, the government announced a further, independent review of existing and new evidence on SP that is to report in March 2014 [29].
Prior to the consultation, the Department of Health (DH) commissioned a systematic review of the evidence base on SP (hereafter referred to as the Moodie review, after the lead author's name), which concluded that there was ‘strong evidence’ that SP would reduce the attractiveness and appeal of tobacco products and increase the effectiveness of health warnings and, therefore, had the potential to ‘contribute to reductions in the harm caused by tobacco smoking’ [30]. The four main TTCs (BAT, Japan Tobacco International (JTI), Philip Morris Limited (PM), and Imperial Tobacco) submitted large volumes of written submissions to the stakeholder consultation. Our initial analysis of these showed that the companies' main collective argument was that SP would not ‘work’ [31]. The TTCs, on the one hand, dismissed the existing evidence base indicating a likely positive impact of SP and, on the other hand, cited a large volume of alternative evidence (77 items) to support their argument that SP would not work. However, only a small number (17) of this alternative body of evidence was directly relevant to SP, and of these, the majority (14) was industry funded/linked and none was peer-reviewed [31]. By contrast, the studies included in the Moodie review were all relevant, the majority were peer-reviewed, and none was industry funded/linked.
Here, we build on this initial overview by providing a detailed examination of how the industry critiqued evidence supporting SP. We extend the current literature on TTCs' misuse and misrepresentation of science, summarised above, by building an in-depth conceptual account of industry arguments and practices used to undermine published evidence. We thus provide insights into the complexities of evidence-based policymaking where corporate interests conflict with public policy goals.
Methods
Data Sources
The four TTCs' submissions to the consultation were published in full on individual company websites. Our initial review of all four TTCs' submissions and coding of the volume, relevance, and quality of all the evidence cited by TTCs highlighted both the large volume of submissions and evidence cited. Submissions from the four TTCs totalled 1521 pages and cited 143 pieces of formal written research evidence [31]. We therefore limited our analysis to two companies, BAT and JTI [32],[33], chosen for the following reasons. BAT has a large market share in countries that have already implemented (Australia) or are considering (e.g., New Zealand) SP, making its strategy of wider interest. JTI has a large market share in the UK and Ireland (also considering SP), yet, unlike BAT and Philip Morris Ltd, was not subject to the litigation that led to document disclosure. With no internal documents available for scrutiny, far less is known about JTI, and analysis of its submission may therefore provide some insights into how the company seeks to influence policy.
We selected for analysis those sections of the BAT and JTI submissions (including expert reports) that focused on whether SP would produce the intended public health objectives (i.e., whether it would work). This represented a core topic in three (BAT, JTI, and PM) of the four TCCs' submissions and accounted for the largest volume of cited evidence (88/143) [31]. BAT and JTI also submitted a total of 14 expert reports covering different aspects of SP (two appended to the BAT submission and 12 submitted separately by JTI) [34],[35],[36],[37],[38],[39],[40],[41],[42],[43],[44],[45],[46],[47]. Of these, six (one BAT, five JTI) authored by three experts related to the core topic of whether SP would work. We selected the one relevant BAT report by Klick [34] and the two most recent JTI reports by Keegan and Devinney [38],[40] for analysis (Table 1).
The reports by Keegan [38] and Devinney [40] critiqued a very large number of published studies on the impacts of SP, many of which were included in the Moodie review; Klick's report [34] was less detailed. We read all three [34],[38],[40] to form an idea of the type of critique offered, ascertaining that the critiques were similar, particularly those of Keegan, a business/marketing professor, and Devinney, an economics/business professor, although Devinney's tended to be more detailed. We therefore selected two of Devinney's study critiques for critical appraisal. Hammond et al, 2011 [48] was an online survey of young women (smokers and non-smokers) randomised to view different pack designs. The main finding was that fully branded female packs had greater appeal and were more likely to be associated with glamour, slimness, attractiveness, and less harm compared to plain packs. Thrasher et al, 2011 [49] conducted experimental auctions among adult smokers, and the main finding was that the lowest bid was associated with plain packs with prominent pictorial health warnings. We selected these study critiques because they contained detailed critiques using a wide variety of statistical and other methodological arguments that were also used for critiquing other studies.
Analysis
We conducted four types of analysis. We examined the presentation of author CVs in all 14 expert reports. We then conducted two types of in-depth analysis on the selected documents (two main submissions and three expert reports): first, a verification-oriented cross-documentary analysis comparing references made to published sources with the original sources to ascertain how they had been used; second, an interpretive analysis to identify conceptual themes. The interpretive analysis was framed by the social constructivist perspectives of social studies of science [50],[51] and the premise that facts, including scientific facts, are socially and interactionally constructed and open to alternative interpretations. The literature on the industry's use and misuse of science (summarised in the Introduction) also formed a sensitising empirical backdrop to the analysis. We used thematic analysis informed by the approach and techniques of constructivist grounded theory [52],[53]: systematic conceptual coding (using Atlas.ti software), constant comparison, discourse sensitivity, attention to divergent data, and conceptual conclusions. One researcher (SU) read and micro-coded selected sections of the documents line-by-line for salient themes. Coding was inductive and emergent, although some themes replicated those identified during the preceding literature review, for example: (ignoring) weight of evidence; diversion (introducing alternative evidence); selective quoting; and wholesale discounting (of evidence). A sub-sample of the material (13%) was coded by two other researchers (JH, GF); there was considerable convergence in the initial analytic themes identified and agreement was reached through discussion on the differences in coding. The micro (first level) themes were grouped under broader categories; for example, the themes ‘misleading quoting’, ‘misleading interpretation’, and ‘selective quoting’ were grouped under ‘misleading quoting of evidence’. Emerging ideas were discussed by the wider team at interim analytic meetings. Finally, we conducted critical appraisal of Devinney's critique [40] of Hammond et al, 2011 [48] and Thrasher et al, 2011 [49]. Two authors (SU, AG) conducted this analysis with comments obtained from the original study authors.
Results
The 15 pages (of 77) in the main BAT submission and 26 pages (of 116) in the JTI submission (41 of 193 pages (21%) combined) that dealt with the question of whether or not SP would work were analysed. Of the three expert reports we selected, we analysed 4 pages of Klick (BAT) [34], 29 pages of Keegan (JTI) [38], and 31 pages of Devinney (JTI) [40].
The BAT and JTI documents make the case that the evidence for SP was seriously and fatally flawed, delivering detailed and mostly externally commissioned critiques of the individual published studies and the Moodie review and reiterating that the DH had relied on ‘insufficient and unreliable’ [32] or ‘unreliable and unconvincing’ [33] evidence while ignoring other relevant evidence [32],[33]. We first look at the size of the submissions and their potentially debilitating effect on policymaking and then detail three industry practices—misleading quoting of evidence, mimicked scientific critique, and evidential landscaping—instrumental in undermining the evidence base for SP (Table 2).
Overwhelming Volume of Submissions
The size of the BAT (77 pages) and JTI (116 pages) submissions was their most immediately noticeable feature; the 12 expert reports that formed part of the JTI submission made up a further 1026 pages and the 2 in the BAT submission made up another 45 pages, bringing the total for the two companies to 1264 pages. In this study, it took one experienced full-time researcher 26 working days to analyse 105 pages, an average of 4 pages per day. If only half the 1521 pages submitted by four TTCs required close analysis, this would take 190 days (eight and a half months) to accomplish. Even a straightforward reading with no attempt at analysis would clearly take a very long time. Given that there were many other submissions from organisations and individuals (2444 in total, excluding brief ‘campaign’ responses [54]), this raises questions over the capacity of public bureaucracies to assimilate, synthesize, and consider the evidence submitted.
Misleading Quoting of Evidence
This was a technique heavily used in the BAT and to a lesser extent in the JTI submissions. It involved inaccurate reporting of objectives, methods, findings, or conclusions of studies; presenting a minor point as a main conclusion; and the ‘tweezers method’ of partially quoting the original source and omitting qualifying information. The effect of all these practices was to distort or even contradict the meanings in the original source with the result that evidence supportive of SP was transformed into evidence against SP. There were 28 instances of this technique in the 15 pages analysed in the BAT submission and 13 instances in the 26 pages analysed in the JTI submission. Eliciting examples of this technique was labour and time intensive as it involved detailed examination of original sources. This technique relied for its effectiveness on the reluctance and/or inability of third parties (bureaucrats, interest groups, academic researchers) to devote time to such scrutiny.
A frequently employed method of inaccurate reporting was conflating absence of evidence with evidence of absence, i.e., arguing that no connection was found between smoking and packaging when the studies concerned had not explored this relationship. For example, in reporting results of a survey examining reasons for quitting [55], BAT claimed that ‘none of the main reasons stated involved packaging’ [32]. But examination of the survey reveals that packaging was not a response option, so it could not be selected as a reason. There were eight other instances of this technique in the BAT and JTI submissions.
BAT argued that, far from increasing the salience of health warnings, SP ‘may actually reduce smokers’ attention to warnings', citing as evidence a study in the Moodie review [56] that compared participants' eye movements when viewing branded and plain packs.
‘The researchers concluded that daily smokers exhibited more eye movements towards health warnings when the pack was branded than when it was plain…’ [32].
This was not the conclusion of the study by Munafo and colleagues [56]. The study concluded that SP had no effect on daily smokers whose attention to warnings remained the same (and did not decrease as suggested by BAT), while it appeared to increase attention to health warnings among non-smokers and light, non-established smokers.
BAT also claimed that the DH had previously concluded that there was not enough evidence to support SP:
‘When the Department of Health examined Plain Packaging … in 2008 it concluded that: “the research evidence into this [plain packaging] initiative is speculative, relying on asking people what they might do in a certain situation”’ [32, emphasis added].
This statement was not, however, a conclusion; the DH document listed five ‘positive effects’ and seven ‘potential disadvantages’ of SP, and the extract BAT quoted was one of the disadvantages [57].
One of JTI's criticisms of the Moodie review was that it had ‘inappropriately’ included studies ‘which exist only in abstract form’, something that would limit thorough critical assessment [33]. Three studies (out of 37) were cited as examples. Yet, despite the methodological challenges, it is considered good practice to include grey literature in systematic reviews in order to reduce publication bias [58],[59],[60]. Furthermore, the review's bibliography shows that for one [61], the authors had access to an associated PhD thesis and for another [62], to a draft/unpublished manuscript; it was not clear whether a full version was available in the third case [63].
JTI also claimed that the review's authors had ignored reports by two JTI experts, Devinney and Keegan, despite the citation of one by Devinney [39] in the review's protocol [64], resulting in the Moodie review being ‘at best flawed, and at worst, skewed’ [33]. Intriguingly, JTI acknowledged (in the same paragraph) that the reports could not have been included in the systematic review as they were secondary reviews and the systematic review only included primary studies. Furthermore, the Devinney report was cited in the Moodie review's protocol as an example of a review that ‘failed to adopt a systematic approach’, confirming the need for a systematic review [64], which JTI failed to mention when making its claim of exclusion.
While not exactly inaccurate reporting, the ‘tweezers method’ misrepresented original studies through selectivity. According to JTI, the Moodie review concluded that studies had failed to show that SP would have a positive impact on smoking behaviours:
‘… [the review] finds that the “overall pattern of findings is mixed”’ [33].
However, this sentence in the review continues: ‘…. mixed, but tends to be supportive of plain packaging being perceived to have a likely deterrent effect on smoking’ [30]. This part of the sentence was omitted from the JTI quotation.
BAT claimed that a US Food and Drug Administration (FDA) document confirmed that graphic warnings (which the Moodie review found would become more salient and effective on standardised packs) did not impact on smoking cessation:
‘… the US FDA Regulatory Impact Assessment estimated that the impact of the graphic warnings on reducing smoking rates was “in general not statistically distinguishable from zero”’ [33].
The FDA document in question relates to the introduction of graphic warnings in the US from September 2012 as a result of ‘… substantial evidence indicating that larger … warnings including a graphic component … would offer significant health benefits over the existing warnings’ [65]. The extract BAT quoted was from a technical appendix (p. 230 of the 231-page document) that explains the statistical uncertainty around the estimates. The full quotation is:
‘Although both of the estimation methods… lead to the conclusion that graphic warning labels will reduce smoking rates, FDA has had access to very small data sets, so our effectiveness estimates are in general not statistically distinguishable from zero; we therefore cannot reject, in a statistical sense, the possibility that the rule will not change the U.S. smoking rate. Therefore, the appropriate lower bound on benefits is zero’ (p. 230) [65].
The BAT submission quoted part of a report by the US Surgeon General [66] as corroboration of the company's argument that ‘packaging is not a relevant factor in driving [smoking]’ and that the ‘real’ drivers were parental and peer influences, risk preferences, socioeconomic factors, access, and price [32]. These factors are indeed highlighted in the Surgeon General's report [66] in Chapter 4, which covers ‘social, environmental, cognitive and genetic influences’ on smoking among youth and young adults. However, Chapter 5 of the report examines ‘the tobacco industry's influences on the use of tobacco among youth’ and concludes that there is ‘sufficient evidence’ of a causal relationship between ‘advertising and promotion’ and ‘initiation and progression’ of tobacco use (p. 10) and that there is ‘suggestive’ but not ‘sufficient’ evidence that tobacco companies have changed packaging and design in ways that have increased the products' ‘appeal to adolescents and young adults’ (p. 10). Chapter 5, in other words, suggests that packaging is a relevant factor in driving smoking, but this chapter is not mentioned by BAT.
Mimicked Scientific Critique
This strategy, central to the JTI submission but also used by BAT, involved the detailed inspection, mainly by commissioned experts, of individual published SP studies and the Moodie review for methodological rigour and value, resulting in the rejection of the entire body of evidence for SP as flawed. This strategy worked at a deeper level because its discovery required broad epistemological understandings of science; specialised expertise in a large number of disciplines, research traditions, and methodologies; and skills in interpretive analysis, unlikely to be common among civil servants. Of the 37 studies in the Moodie review, 23 were subjected to parallel review across five JTI expert reports [36],[37],[38],[39],[40] (although, for two of these, unpublished versions were critiqued in industry reports). All 23 reported findings supportive of SP and all 23 were found to be ‘methodologically flawed and unreliable’; by contrast, in the Moodie review, 21 were rated as ‘medium’ and two as ‘high’ quality [30]. The industry experts found the Moodie review itself to be similarly flawed and unreliable. In reaching this conclusion, the experts appeared to use scientific methods of critique but close examination of the critiques showed that these were embedded in an essentially unscientific paradigm. They superficially resembled scientific peer review in that they declared the approach and evaluative criteria used in reviewing, identified methodologically problematic aspects of the studies (relating to design, data collection, and analysis) and explained how these might be overcome, and assessed the overall ‘reliability’ of the studies and whether their conclusions were justified by the reported findings. In doing so, the reviews used scientific terminology, even what might be called jargon. However, we identify and describe four features that marked these industry reviews as inauthentic and rendered them a ‘mimicked’ version of scientific critique: seeking methodological perfection; insisting on methodological uniformity; lack of rigour; and adopting the litigation model (Table 2).
Seeking methodological perfection
As SP is not widely implemented, observational evidence of its effects on smoking is not available. Research has therefore focused on three psycho-social mechanisms through which SP is likely to reduce smoking rates: reducing the appeal of packs and products; increasing the salience and effectiveness of warnings; and reducing misperceptions of product strength and harm [30]. This approach—obtaining the best available evidence within contextual limitations—is a key feature of scientific research [67],[68] but was vigorously rejected by the industry and its experts. BAT argued that mechanisms could only be considered once there was evidence of an actual impact on smoking behaviours, failing to acknowledge that the reverse was just as legitimate a scientific objective. JTI similarly rejected the legitimacy and value of mechanisms research.
Absence of ‘real world evidence’ [34], ‘actual behavioural outcomes’ [40], and correspondence between experimental results and ‘actual consumer behaviour’ [39] were repeated criticisms in industry experts' critiques of the SP literature. Studies were dismissed because they did not examine actual cigarette purchasing and smoking decisions or because experimental tasks did not replicate the real world. For example, experiments conducted in an environment where not all packages were plain could not be used to predict purchasing behaviours under a SP regime when all packaging would be plain [34],[38],[40]. Clearly, experiments are, by their nature, artificial and are often conducted because examining real-life behaviours and events is not feasible.
Because observational evidence cannot be obtained without the introduction of SP (as JTI acknowledged in its submission) [33], JTI, BAT, and industry experts effectively locked themselves into a nihilistic position that could potentially prevent the introduction of SP indefinitely: SP could not be introduced without evidence that it changed actual smoking behaviours; this evidence could not be obtained without the introduction of SP; and since SP could not be introduced without this evidence, SP could never be introduced. This premise was also future-proofed against emerging evidence from the Australian experience with SP:
‘… even if [SP legislation in Australia] survives legal challenges … differences between markets may confound comparisons or the transposition of effects’ [33].
It was suggested that the ‘ideal’ SP study, in line with ‘proper research design’, would be a randomised controlled trial that randomised different cities over a ‘long time period’ to having/not having SP [34]. This is clearly an unfeasible proposition not least because population mobility would mean that both groups have access to branded and standardised packages, rendering the results meaningless.
Perfectionism was also evident in the industry's representation of research limitations: the presence of limitations (regardless of nature, severity, and the extent to which they impact on the validity of findings) indicated wholesale failure of a study. All studies, including the best designed and conducted, are subject to some limitations, but one industry expert claimed that it was possible to design and conduct research with no limitations. While acknowledging that ‘no study was perfect’, he declared that ‘a well designed and executed study can avoid and/or correct for such limitations’ [38]. Having built this argument, the industry experts single-mindedly focused on identifying every conceivable limitation of the studies they reviewed (using narrowly defined criteria as explained in the following section, Insisting on methodological uniformity), while remaining silent on their strengths (Tables 3 and 4). Consequently, not one of the studies reviewed was found to provide ‘reliable’ evidence that SP would be effective [40], despite the vast majority having been published in peer-reviewed journals. Even one study that most closely fitted the experts' model of research and met virtually all their criteria, that by Thrasher and colleagues [49], was dismissed as ‘unreliable’ (Table 4).
This view of limitations appeared to be operative in the industry's critique of the Moodie review, along with an apparent failure to understand the logic of synthesis:
‘Adding together multiple flawed studies does not make reliable an unreliable study … students who fail one exam do not have their position improved if they fail multiple exams’ [33].
‘Consistent results from studies that uniformly have the same methodological problems provide zero confidence in any conclusion except, perhaps, that the research designs were flawed in consistent ways’ [34].
In evidence synthesis, limitations of included studies are dealt with in two ways: studies judged to be below a quality threshold due to severe limitations are excluded; and the limitations of the included studies are taken into account through weighting and interpretation of results [58].
Perfectionism also led to suggestions that would breach research governance requirements. Devinney's insistence on studying actual purchasing outcomes or incentive-compatible measures [40], for example, would likely necessitate the exposure of young non-smokers (given that this is the target group of interest in standardised packaging) to attractive cigarette packs, a study design unlikely to receive ethical approval given what is already known on the impact of tobacco marketing (e.g., see Table 3). Keegan [38] and Devinney [40] argue that participants should not be informed of the purpose and sponsor of the research because this information would bias their responses. Failure to provide this information would breach ethics requirements in both social and health research, where researchers are obliged to inform potential participants of the purpose, methods, uses, and funding of the research [69],[70].
Demanding the perfect study design regardless of real-life and methodological constraints is clearly an unscientific position, at least outside the laboratory, but this was the premise of much industry critique of published SP studies, with the result that the reviewed evidence base for SP was discounted.
Insisting on methodological uniformity
The industry critiques of SP studies and the Moodie review were all framed by the discipline of market research and associated methodological conventions. The experts' disciplinary backgrounds included law, marketing, psychology, management, economics, and statistics; none declared expertise in tobacco control or public health. Keegan and Devinney reported using a set of assessment criteria for ‘good research’, citing mainly market research or public opinion research organisations as sources. Discrete choice experiments were designated as the best model for experiments involving purchasing decisions [40].These narrow standards were then used to systematically dismiss any study that did not fit the market research model, failing to recognise that other study types were valid and that many of the research objectives of the studies undertaken could not have been met via such a research model (Table 3).
Furthermore, the quantitative paradigm was accepted as the only scientifically acceptable one and qualitative methodology was dismissed, based on quantitative criteria, as useless.
‘… eight of the studies [in the Moodie review] were little more than small sample focus groups and two other studies were interview based; yet these were considered to be equally valid to other studies in the discussion’ [33].
‘Most of the normal checks that one attributes to good research practice … are violated [in focus groups]. One cannot control for truthfulness or incentive compatibility nor can the focus group questioning be subject to statistical analysis …’ [40].
In social science research, focus groups are not conducted to obtain statistical or generalisable findings but to elicit attitudes, beliefs, and experiences in a relatively naturalistic, informal manner [71]; their rigour is assessed using method-specific concepts and criteria. Qualitative studies, including focus groups, are increasingly recognised (e.g., by the UK's Cochrane Collaboration) as integral components in good quality evidence syntheses [72].
The Moodie review was dismissed as ‘the considered opinion of the author undertaking the review’ and ‘a focus group of researchers giving their opinion’ [33] because it used (qualitative) narrative synthesis rather than (quantitative) meta-analysis due to heterogeneity of the studies. Although challenging, narrative synthesis is an established method with authoritative guidelines available for its conduct [58]. The industry's criticism also appears to confuse a systematic review that uses narrative synthesis (e.g., the Moodie review) with a narrative, non-systematic, review [34].
The industry experts' methodological approach to reviewing the SP evidence base represents a fundamental failure to understand the requirement for methodological pluralism. A key indicator of quality in all research is the fit between the research question and the method(s) because no one paradigm or method can answer all types of research questions.
Lack of rigour
A central tenet of scientific review is diligence, and reviewers take great care to acquire a full and accurate understanding of the original sources they are reviewing; they use a disciplined and methodical approach, applying evaluative criteria consistently across studies to prevent biased judgment. In reviewing the two SP papers by Hammond et al [48] and Thrasher et al [49], JTI expert Devinney [40] appeared to disregard study objectives, experimental processes, analytic strategies, and steps taken by the original authors to minimise the impact of limitations on findings (Tables 3 and 4). In the main TTC submissions, as well as expert reviews, evaluative criteria were used inconsistently, leading in some instances to the use of double standards. For example, despite the industry's criticisms of self-reported beliefs, attitudes, experiences, and intentions in SP research, other studies that used these methods were promoted as good evidence because the results favoured the industry's case [32]. The TTCs criticised SP studies for jumping from data on particular populations to universal ‘speculations’ but this was exactly what the BAT submission did in arguing that SP might generate a ‘forbidden fruit’ effect and increase youth uptake of smoking [32]. This argument was based on a single study [73], the authors of which warned that its findings were ‘difficult to generalise and extrapolate’. The phenomenon of pack ‘appeal’, one mechanism linked to smoking uptake and therefore studied by public health researchers, was condemned as an ‘amorphous and vague concept … lacking in any evidential foundation … and arbitrary’ and could not justify the introduction of SP, JTI argued [33]. But JTI expert Devinney used the term liberally without objecting to it in his report [40] and Keegan referred to it in describing attitudinal research, for example to determine the appeal of detergent packaging [38].
One industry criticism of the Moodie review was that it was ‘inherently biased and self-interested’ [33] because its authors were ‘proponents and advocates of plain packaging’ [32] who worked together and ‘recycled’ information and methodologies [32]. This argument indicates a misunderstanding of scientific work. Evidence synthesis requires the collaboration of scientists/academics with a range of relevant expertise [58] working in the same or related fields and whose work is subject to peer-review. Furthermore, this criticism disregards the fact that the industry experts produced mainly single-author reports at the behest of the tobacco companies (although some declared independence) that were not peer-reviewed.
Adopting the litigation model
The industry reviews appeared to be embedded within a litigation—not scientific—model. Some industry experts referred to parts of their reports that would normally be labelled ‘appendix’ or ‘chapter’ as ‘exhibits’, and their critiques resembled courtroom testimonies aimed at demolishing the adversary's case. In the main TTC submissions, the experts were posited as sources of higher scientific authority, representing ‘the best contemporary scientific thinking’ [33] and were cited extensively. Demonstrating credibility was an important part of this project, with author CVs in the expert reports ranging between 10 and 20 pages and in one instance taking up 61 pages of a 98-page report [41]. Privileging the individual expert is a legal phenomenon and the legitimacy ascribed to individual experts' testimonies in the courts is fundamentally different from collectively established and consensus-based scientific legitimacy developed within specialised ‘communities of practice’ [74]. The tobacco company commissioned experts, working outside the peer-review system, dismissed the (peer-reviewed) evidence base for SP as flawed and unusable. In doing so, it was clear that they were attempting to establish an alternative system of scientific legitimacy.
Another manifestation of the litigation model was the experts' piece-by-piece approach to reviewing. Individual studies were examined in depth to determine whether any—on its own—constituted a warrant for SP and, following systematic deconstruction, none was found to be good enough to justify SP.
‘In summary … it is my expert opinion that none of the Studies provide [sic] reliable evidence that plain packaging would be effective in achieving the public policy goals of changing actual smoking behaviour …’ [40].
In court, each piece of evidence (i.e., each study and the Moodie review) is treated as a separate piece of evidence and each needs to be undermined and discredited in turn until no evidence remains that could damage one's client's case. By contrast, in scientific work, it is essential that the extant research is synthesised and greater confidence in the findings established through the cumulative ‘weight of the evidence’ [75].
To sum up, the two TTC submissions (by BAT and JTI) and the associated expert reports used different combinations of the techniques of mimicked critique we have documented here to dismiss the entire literature supportive of SP.
Evidential Landscaping
As well as dismissing research that supported SP, another, more expansive, strategy in the submissions was to change the evidential landscape within which the policy debate was conducted by introducing and promoting research that examined non-packaging issues and, at the same time, excluding relevant industry research on packaging.
Promoting alternative evidence
The industry pointed to the literature on psychological and social explanations of smoking behaviours and individual decision-making to argue that packaging was irrelevant to smoking behaviours and decisions. The DH and the Moodie review were said to ‘ignore’ this parallel evidence base.
‘… the real drivers of smoking initiation include factors such as parental influences, risk preferences, peer influences, socioeconomic factors, access and price’ [32].
‘Numerous government funded and independent studies also show that factors other than packaging are the real drivers of decisions relating to quitting and relapse’ [33].
JTI submitted two expert reports to support this argument. One was prepared by two marketing professors who drew on the literatures on ‘consumer, cognitive and social psychology, behavioural economics and marketing research’ and concluded that SP would be ineffective because smoking decisions and behaviours were made on the basis of habit, goals and motives, peer influences, the consumer mindset, and self-control [42]. The other, by a psychology professor, argued changes in packaging did not affect ‘adolescents’ experimentation with or use of cigarettes', which were based instead on sensation-seeking, peer and family influences, a focus on rewards rather than risks, and availability [41]. While this body of research clearly makes an important contribution to knowledge on smoking behaviours, it overlooks the overwhelming evidence that tobacco marketing also plays a key role in shaping smoking behaviour [76] and cannot be legitimately used to argue that SP will prove to be an ineffective tobacco control measure. Smoking uptake is a complex phenomenon with multiple explanatory factors acting at both individual and population levels—only some of which are amenable to intervention. It is also a basic tenet of public health that population-level interventions, such as SP, by virtue of reaching the whole population, have a greater impact than individual-level interventions [77], yet, the majority of interventions promoted by the experts as more effective alternatives to SP were targeted at individuals.
Excluding relevant internal tobacco industry research
The DH consultation brief included a request for respondents to provide ‘any further evidence about tobacco packaging that you believe to be helpful’ [78]. Industry documents previously made public as a result of litigation and other more recent documentation [79] show that packaging is central to current marketing efforts and that the industry funds ‘systematic and extensive’ internal research on packaging [80]. This research covers RJ Reynolds (the international division of which was later taken over by JTI), Philip Morris, Brown & Williamson, American Tobacco Company, Lorillard Tobacco Company [80],[81],[82], and British American Tobacco Company [80],[82]. It shows that pack style can impact on smokers' perception of cigarette taste [80]; can (misleadingly) suggest that the contents carry lower health risks because they are portrayed as ‘milder’ or ‘lighter’ than other brands [82]; and is used by companies to increase product appeal to targeted groups, including young people [81],[82]. This body of evidence is therefore at least as—if not more—relevant to standardised packaging as the psycho-social body of evidence on smoking decision-making promoted by the TTCs in their submissions. It is clear both from this study and our earlier work [31] that none of the four TTCs referred to internal tobacco industry research on packaging in their submissions, despite citing a very large volume of other evidence to support their arguments. We do not know whether internal packaging research was shared with the DH on a confidential basis. However, the industry's public argument that packaging does not impact on smoking behaviours clearly lacks the benefit of the industry's own research on packaging.
Discussion
We have reported the methods BAT and JTI used in their written submissions to undermine the evidence base for SP. The companies submitted a very large volume of evidence, reported studies in an inaccurate and misleading fashion, and sought to diminish the value and exaggerate the shortcomings of studies by using mimicked scientific critique. This last technique involved judging published studies against unrealistic and perfectionist criteria, dismissing research traditions and methods outside a narrowly defined paradigm (e.g., quantitative marketing research), adopting a litigation style that contrasted with the scientific model, and using non-rigorous review practices. In addition, they disregarded the consistency in the evidence for SP and tried to divert attention away from packaging to an alternative, less relevant body of evidence, while excluding a highly relevant body of industry research on packaging. These practices were conducted under the rubric of scientific critique and gave a misleading impression of scientific credibility. Our analysis suggests that they amount instead to an attempt to create a parallel and competing ‘scientific’ discourse. In formulating their response to the stakeholder consultation on SP, the tobacco companies reframed prevailing scientific norms and practices as somehow substandard and corrupt, seeking to impress on policy makers their own—distorted—interpretation of science and the scientific method.
It is not possible to ascertain how successful the tobacco industry was in influencing the UK government's stance on SP. Nevertheless, the government's announcement that it was postponing a decision due to concerns about the evidence base, combined with our findings and the industry's record in manipulating evidence to create policy inertia [83], suggest that industry plans to contest the evidence base [84],[85] met with some success, at least initially. The fact that this occurred despite the favourable conclusions of the Moodie review and strong evidence of the role of packaging in marketing [80],[81],[82] is notable.
This study had a number of limitations. Our analysis covered just two of the four tobacco company submissions. However, content analysis of how evidence was cited in all four submissions [31] suggests that our findings may be applicable to the other two TTCs, while leaked documents [84],[85] show Philip Morris International (PMI) also intended to contest the evidence base for SP. Additionally, we only focused on those sections dealing with whether SP would achieve the intended public health outcomes; we did not analyse sections on unintended consequences (economic, illicit trade, legal) that also cite and present different types of evidence. More generally, our research represents only one side of a dyadic phenomenon: the effectiveness of TTC attempts at influencing public health policy depends on how policymakers respond to such attempts. As intimated above, our current research is unable to examine this issue in any detail.
Nevertheless, our work provides crucial insights into how TTCs seek to discredit the evidence for a policy that runs counter to their interests and how regulatory tools associated with the Better Regulation agenda facilitate such efforts. In relation to the former, our findings are consistent with a broad body of literature on industry misuse of science and promotion of ‘sound science’. This literature shows that industries attempt to prevent or delay regulatory action aimed at limiting or removing harms associated with their products by creating doubt about the validity of scientific evidence that documents those harms. This extends from the tobacco industry [9],[10],[11],[13],[86] to dioxin (a synthetic carcinogen) [87], aspirin (in its association with Reye's syndrome), [12] and, more generally, to genetically modified corn (and its potential to spread in the wild) [88], acid rain, and global warming [8]. A recent study found that the alcohol industry adopted a similar strategy to the TTCs' in its submissions to the Scottish government's 2008 consultation on alcohol policies and minimum pricing [5]. So extensive is the practice of creating doubt and denying knowledge that Proctor has coined the term ‘agnotology’ (the cultural production of ignorance and its study) to describe, inter alia, the tobacco industry's manufacture of politically motivated ignorance in its attempt to transform evidential uncertainty into a paralysing search for certainty and subsequent policy inaction [83].
It is important to understand that the socially constructed nature of scientific work itself [50],[89] makes its manipulation by corporate interests possible. Science can be understood as a negotiated order sustained by craft conventions that are formulated, maintained, and altered over time within communities of practice. Methodological choices and compromises need to be made in response to contingencies; surrogate end points (e.g., mechanisms in SP research) are used when necessary; boundaries of acceptable limitations are defined; and different epistemologies (e.g., experimental, quantitative, and qualitative methods) are not only tolerated but put to productive use singly and in combination. Scientific knowledge is ‘simply the best knowledge available to a particular community working in a particular paradigm, with particular assumptions, instruments, and techniques’ [90]. This social nature of science constitutes a soft underbelly open to attack by commercial interests—tobacco and other—in their efforts to shape policy. A number of indicators have been suggested to help distinguish genuine scientific critique from the mimicked version. Two indicators suggested by de Camargo [91] are: ‘follow the money’, i.e., industry funding of the critique; and the intended audience, i.e., not scientific but public, legal, and regulatory communities. We concur with both. Our earlier work highlighted the significance of funding and independence [31]. For all the resources deployed by tobacco companies to undermine the SP evidence base, our analysis suggests that the industry's deconstruction was not particularly sophisticated from a scientific perspective. This may be because it was not intended to be persuasive to the scientific community, but was designed to speak to politicians, bureaucrats, and ultimately, in the context of judicial review, to judges. A third indicator, suggested by Jasanoff and again confirmed by our data, is disciplinary affiliation and perspective. Jasanoff points out that industry-commissioned experts, who ‘sow reasonable doubt about the reliability of particular scientific practices’, are often from different scientific ‘subcultures’ to those in which the original work was produced [92]. They use ‘idealised norms’ of scientific practice, deploy an ‘inherently unforgiving’ perspective, and their objective is to discredit by ‘exposing gaps and omissions’. Genuine scientific critique, on the other hand, is typically conducted by those located within the same scientific subculture (discipline/specialty) because only they ‘may be truly in a position to evaluate each other's competence’ and is aimed at improving the work so that it can contribute to ‘the larger enterprise of creating new knowledge’. Jasanoff's observation implies a fourth indicator—motivation. The TTCs' critiques were not intended, and did not originate, as contributions to the stock of scientific knowledge on SP but were produced in response to the public consultation on SP to support the TTC argument that SP should not be introduced.
While the tobacco industry has a long history of challenging scientific evidence in order to resist public health policies, the Better Regulation agenda, with its emphasis on evidence and requirement for stakeholder consultation, increases the utility of this strategy. This is something the TTCs recognise. Previous work shows that BAT predicted that the implementation of Better Regulation would enhance its ability to prevent public health policies [2]. Recently leaked PMI documents outlining the company's plans to prevent the introduction of SP in the UK suggest that PMI determined that ‘evidence based policy’, an invitation to ‘wait and see what happens in Australia’, and an emphasis on ‘Better Regulation’ should be central to its campaign along with the argument that SP would lead to increased illicit trade, adverse impacts on trade, and legal issues [84]. PMI also identified ‘evidence based argumentation’ as a strength of its corporate strategy in opposing SP and the prospect of ‘government ignoring Better Regulation principles’ as a threat to it [85]. The twin political imperative—to base policies and regulation on scientific evidence and, at the same time, to democratise science by inviting review from stakeholders—provides significant opportunities for tobacco and other corporate interests to challenge and obstruct regulation by challenging and undermining the evidence behind it. Questions therefore inevitably arise concerning the scientific value and public interest implications of subjecting evidence produced by non-commercially-affiliated academic researchers who have satisfied the increasingly elaborate and rigorous demands of public funding agencies, peer-review, and research governance systems to repeated episodes of review by those with a vested interest. Placing a formal obligation on civil servants to seek ‘stakeholders’ views on … key assumptions and data’ [3] arguably amounts to a vote of no or little confidence in the painstakingly constructed and expensive edifice of public research.
A further problem presented is the question of how such reviews are then handled by civil servants and politicians. Despite calls for transparency [93], the use of evidence in policymaking remains a black box. We do not know, for example, how closely the evidential critiques submitted by BAT and JTI were read and analysed and how much weight was placed on them. Without spending considerable time (given the volume of the TTC submissions) and without relevant scientific skills and knowledge, civil servants would have little choice but to take the industry's scientific critiques at face value. Alternatively, considerable public resources would need to be spent on analysis and verification by external experts.
Conclusion
This study shows that the TTCs' critique of the evidence in favour of SP is highly misleading. Combined with our earlier work highlighting the very low quality of the TTCs' evidence against SP, it suggests that the TTCs' claim that SP will not lead to public health benefits is largely without foundation. It also highlights that the tools of Better Regulation, particularly stakeholder consultation, just as BAT predicted when pushing for their implementation, provide an opportunity for highly resourced corporations to slow, weaken, or prevent public health policies.
Zdroje
1. Smokefree Partnership (2010) The origin of EU Better Regulation - The Disturbing Truth. Brussels: the Smokefree Partnership.
2. SmithKE, FooksG, CollinJ, WeishaarH, MandalS, et al. (2010) “Working the system”—British American Tobacco's influence on the European Union Treaty and its implications for policy: An analysis of internal tobacco industry documents. PLoS Med 7 (1) e1000202 doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.1000202.
3. DBIS. Department for Business Innovation & Skills (2013) Better Regulation Framework Manual. London: DBIS.
4. HM Treasury (2003) The green book: Appraisal and evaluation in central government. London: TSO.
5. McCambridgeJ, HawkinsB, HoldenC (2013) Industry use of evidence to influence alcohol policy: A case study of submissions to the 2008 Scottish government consultation. PLoS Med 10 (4) e1001431 doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.1001431.
6. Crowe K (2013) Sugar industry's secret documents echo tobacco tactics: Sugar Association's intent to use science to defeat critics uncovered by dentist. CBC News. Available: http://www.cbc.ca/news/health/sugar-industry-s-secret-documents-echo-tobacco-tactics-1.1369231. Accessed 12 March 2014.
7. Al-AwqatiQ (2006) Evidence-based politics of salt and blood pressure. Kidney International 69: 1707–1708.
8. Oreskes N, Conway E (2010) Merchants of doubt: How a handful of scientists obscured the truth on issues from tobacco smoke to global warming. New York: Bloomsbury Press.
9. HirschhornN (2000) Shameful science: four decades of the German tobacco industry's hidden research on smoking and health. Tobacco Control 9: 242–247.
10. FriedmanLC, DaynardRA, BanthinCN (2005) Howe tobacco-friendly science escapes scrutiny in the courtroom. American Journal of Public Health 95: S16–S20.
11. OngEK, GlantzSA (2001) Constructing “sound science” and “good epidemiology”: Tobacco, lawyers, and public relations firms. American Journal of Public Health 91: 1749–1757.
12. MichaelsD, MonfortonC (2005) Manufacturing uncertainty: Contested science and the protection of the public's health and environment. American Journal of Public Health 95: S39–S48.
13. FrancisJA, SheaAK, SametJM (2006) Challenging the epidemiologic evidence on passive smoking: tactics of tobacco industry expert witnesses. Tobacco Control 15: 68–76.
14. SmithKE, FooksG, CollinJ, WeishaarH, GilmoreA (2010) Is the increasing policy use of Impact Assessment in Europe likely to undermine efforts to achieve healthy public policy? J Epidemiol Community Health 64 (6) 478–487 Available: http://jech.bmj.com/content/64/6/478.full.pdf.
15. BeroLA (2005) Tobacco industry manipulation of research. Public Health Reports 120: 200–208.
16. SchotlandMS, BeroLA (2002) Evaluating public commentary and scientific evidence submitted in the development of a risk assessment. Risk Analysis 22: 131–140.
17. TongEK, GlantzSA (2007) Tobacco industry efforts undermining evidence linking secondhand smoke with cardiovascular disease. Circulation 116: 1845–1854.
18. WagnerW (2005) The perils of relying on interested parties to evaluate scientific quality. American Journal of Public Health 95: S99–S106.
19. WyniaMK (2005) Judging public health research: Epistemology, public health and the law. American Journal of Bioethics 5: 4–7.
20. GruningT, GilmoreAB, McKeeM (2006) Tobacco industry influence on science and scientists in Germany. American Journal of Public Health 96: 20–32.
21. BeroLA, MontiniT, Bryan-JonesK, MangurianC (2001) Science in regulatory policy making: case studies in the development of workplace smoking restrictions. Tobacco Control 10: 329–336.
22. Physicians for a smoke-free Canada website. Timeline of events related to plain packaging. Available: http://www.smoke-free.ca/plain-packaging/history.htm. Accessed 6 November 2013.
23. Australian Government (2011) Tobacco Plain Packaging Act 2011: An Act to discourage the use of tobacco products, and for related purposes.
24. FooksG, GilmoreAB (2013) International trade law, plain packaging and tobacco industry political activity: the Trans-Pacific Partnership. Tobacco Control Published Online First: 20 June 2013. doi: 10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2012-050869.
25. TobaccoTactics.org. Australia: Challenging Legislation. Available: http://www.tobaccotactics.org/index.php/Australia:_Challenging_Legislation. Accessed 11 November 2013.
26. House of Commons (2013) Plain packaging of tobacco products. In: Section HA, editor. London: House of Commons Library.
27. BBC (2013) Government rejects Labour's cigarette ‘U-turn’ claim. Available: http://www.bbc.co.uk/news/health-23281804. Accessed 12 March 2014.
28. Sparrow A (2013) Anger as plain cigarette packaging plans put on hold by ministers. The Guardian. Available: http://www.theguardian.com/business/2013/jul/12/plain-cigarette-packaging-anger-ministers. Accessed 12 March 2014.
29. BBC (2013) Cigarette packaging: Ministers launch fresh review. Available: http://www.bbc.co.uk/news/uk-politics-25126673. Accessed 12 March 2014.
30. Moodie C, Stead M, Bauld L, McNeill A, Hinds K, et al. (2012) Plain tobacco packaging: a systematic review. Stirling: Public Health Research Consortium. Available: http://phrc.lshtm.ac.uk/papers/PHRC_006_Final_Report.pdf.
31. Hatchard JL, Fooks GJ, Evans-Reeves KA, Ulucanlar S, Gilmore AB (2013) A critical evaluation of the volume, relevance and quality of evidence submitted by the tobacco industry to oppose standardised packaging of tobacco products. BMJ Open 4 (2) e003757. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2013-003757.
32. British American Tobacco (BAT) (2012) UK Standardised Packaging Consultation: Response of British American Tobacco UK Limited. Available: http://www.bat.com/group/sites/uk__3mnfen.nsf/vwPagesWebLive/DO8WZC5E/$FILE/medMD8WZC6J.pdf?openelement.
33. Japan Tobacco International (JTI) (2012) Response to the Department of Health's consultation on the standardised packaging of tobacco products. Available: http://www.jti.com/how-we-do-business/key-regulatory-submissions/.
34. British American Tobacco (BAT) (2012) UK Standardised Appendix A, Report of Dr. Jonathan Klick. Appendix A. Available: http://www.bat.com/group/sites/uk__3mnfen.nsf/vwPagesWebLive/DO8WZC5E/$FILE/medMD8WZC6J.pdf?openelement.
35. British American Tobacco (BAT) (2012) UK Standardised Appendix B, Report of Mr. Stephen Gibson. Appendix B. Available: http://www.bat.com/group/sites/uk__3mnfen.nsf/vwPagesWebLive/DO8WZC5E/$FILE/medMD8WZC6J.pdf?openelement.
36. Keegan W (2008) Analysis of consumer survey evidence relevant to the UK Department of Health consultation on the Future of Tobacco Control. New York. Available: http://www.jti.com/files/7613/4122/2707/Keegan_analysis_uk_DoH.pdf.
37. Keegan W (2009) Analysis of consumer survey evidence relevant to the UK Department of Health consultation on the future of tobacco control: supplemental report. New York. Available: http://www.jti.com/files/4113/4122/2720/Keegan_analysis_uk_DoH_suppl.pdf.
38. Keegan W (2010) Analysis of consumer survey evidence relevant to DG SANCO's proposal to increase the size of health warnings on tobacco packaging. New York. Available: http://www.jti.com/files/1813/4122/2695/Keegan.pdf
39. Devinney T (2010) Analysis of consumer research evidence on the impact of plain packaging for tobacco products. Available: http://www.jti.com/files/3313/3164/0525/Devinney.pdf.
40. Devinney T (2012) Analysis of consumer research evidence on the impact of plain packaging for tobacco products (updated to 2012). Available: http://www.jti.com/files/9813/4149/4426/Prof_Devinney.pdf.
41. Steinberg L (2010) Adolescent decision making and the prevention of underage smoking. Philadelphia, PA. Available: http://www.jti.com/files/2013/4122/2619/Steinberg.pdf.
42. Dhar R, Nowlis S (2010) Report on the consumer behaviour and decision making of cigarette smokers. Available: http://www.jti.com/files/8413/4122/2636/Dhar_Nowlis.pdf.
43. Lilico A (2008) Economic analysis of a display ban and/or a plain packs requirement in the UK: a report from Europe Economics. London. Available: http://www.jti.com/files/2513/3164/0608/Lilico_display_ban_req_uk.pdf.
44. Lilico A (2010) Economic analysis of restrictions on the display of tobacco products - 2009 Canadian annual smoking data. Geneva. Available: http://www.jti.com/files/5513/3164/0566/Lilico_restrictions_CASD.pdf.
45. Cave M (2010) ‘Better Regulation’ and certain tobacco control measures. Available: http://www.jti.com/files/8813/4122/2664/Cave.pdf.
46. Gervais D (2010) Analysis of the compatibility of certain tobacco product packaging rules with the TRIPS agreement and the Paris Convention. Available: http://www.jti.com/files/8513/4122/2680/Gervais.pdf.
47. Chaudhry P, Zimmerman A (2012) The impact of plain packaging on the illicit trade in tobacco products. Available: http://www.jti.com/files/5113/4150/5828/Impact_on_illicit_trade.pdf.
48. HammondD, DoxeyJ, DanielS, Bansal-TraversM (2011) Impact of female-oriented cigarette packaging in the United States. Nicotine & Tobacco Research 13: 579–588 Available: http://ntr.oxfordjournals.org/content/13/7/579.short.
49. ThrasherJF, RousuMC, HammondD, NavarroA, CorriganJR (2011) Estimating the impact of pictorial health warnings and “plain” cigarette packaging: Evidence from experimental auctions among adult smokers in the United States. Health Policy 102: 41–48 Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21763026.
50. Latour B, Woolgar S (1986) Laboratory Life: the construction of scientific facts. New Jersey: Princeton University Press.
51. Bloor D (1991) Knowledge and Social Imagery. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
52. Glaser BG, Strauss AL (1967) The discovery of grounded theory: strategies for qualitative research. New Brunswick: Aldine Transactions.
53. Charmaz K (2009) Shifting the grounds: constructivist grounded theory methods. In: Morse JM, Noerager Stern P, Corbin J, Bowers B, Charmaz K, et al., editors. Developing Grounded Theory. Walnut Creek, California: Left Coast Press. pp. 127–193.
54. Department of Health Tobacco Programme (2013) Consultation on standardised packaging of tobacco products: Summary report. London: Department of Health.
55. Office for National Statistics (ONS) (2008–9) Opinions Survey Report No 40: Smoking related behaviour and attitudes 2008–09. London: ONS. Available: http://www.ons.gov.uk/ons/publications/index.html?newquery=*&newoffset=50&pageSize=50&nscl=Health+and+Social+Care&content-type=Report&content-type-orig=publicationContentTypesOriginal&sortBy=pubdate&sortDirection=DESCENDING&pubdateRangeType=last5yrs&applyFilters=true.
56. MunafoMR, RobertsN, BauldL, LeonardsU (2011) Plain packaging increases visual attention to health warnings on cigarette packs in non-smokers and weekly smokers but not daily smokers. Addiction 106: 1505–1510 Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21401767.
57. Department of Health (2008) Consultation on the future of tobacco control. London: Department of Health. Available: http://webarchive.nationalarchives.gov.uk/+/www.dh.gov.uk/en/Consultations/Responsestoconsultations/DH_091382.
58. Centres for Review and Dissemination (2009) Systematic reviews: CRD's guidance for undertaking reviews in healthcare. York: University of York. Available: http://www.york.ac.uk/inst/crd/pdf/Systematic_Reviews.pdf.
59. Cochrane Collaboration (2011) Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions. Available: http://handbook.cochrane.org/front_page.htm. Part two. Accessed 31 January 2014.
60. BenziesKM, PremjiS, HaydenKA, SerrettK (2006) State-of-the-evidence reviews: advantages and challenges of including grey literature. Worldviews Evid Based Nurs 3 (2) 55–61.
61. White C, Hammond D (2011) The impact of cigarette package design and plain packaging on female youth in Brazil: brand appeal and health-related perceptions. Public Health in Canada: Innovative partnerships for action, Canadian Public Health Association (CPHA) 2011 Conference. Montreal, Canada: CPHA.
62. HammondD, DanielS (2011) UK plain pack study among young women. Under review with peer-reviewed journal
63. Gallopel-Morvan K, Moodie C, Rey J (2010) Demarketing cigarettes through plain cigarette packaging. Actes du Congres International de l'AFM. Le Mans, France.
64. Moodie C, Hastings G, Thomas J, Stead M, Angus K, et al. (2011) Protocol for plain tobacco packaging: a systematic review. Available: http://phrc.lshtm.ac.uk/papers/PHRC_006_Protocol.pdf.
65. US Food and Drug Administration (2011) Required warnings for cigarette packages and advertisements; Final Rule. Federal Register 76 (120) Available: http://www.fda.gov/downloads/TobaccoProducts/GuidanceComplianceRegulatoryInformation/UCM339834.pdf.
66. US Department of Health and Human Services (DHHS) (2012) Preventing tobacco use among youth and young adults: a report of the surgeon general. Atlanta, GA: Centres for Disease Control and Prevention. Available: http://www.cdc.gov/tobacco/data_statistics/sgr/2012/.
67. Yates SJ (2004) Doing social science research. London: Sage Publications.
68. Terre Blanche MJ, Durrheim K, Painter D (2006) Research in Practice: Applied Methods for the Social Sciences. 2nd ed. Cape Town: University of Cape Town Press.
69. ESRC Framework for research ethics. Available: http://www.esrc.ac.uk/_images/Framework-for-Research-Ethics_tcm8-4586.pdf. Accessed 31 January 2014.
70. National Patient Safety Agency. Information sheets and consent form – guidance for researchers and reviewers. Available: http://www.hra.nhs.uk/documents/2013/09/information-sheet-and-consent-form-guidance.pdf. Accessed 31 January 2014.
71. Silverman D (2009) Qualitative Research: theory, method and practice. London: Sage Publications.
72. Noyes J, Popay J, Pearson A, Hannes K, Booth A (2008) Qualitative research and Cochrane reviews; Chapter 20, In: Higgins JPT, Green S (editors) Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions. Version 5.0.1. The Cochrane Collaboration, 2008. Available: http://www.cochrane-handbook.org.
73. SussmanS, GranaR, PokhrelP, RohrbachLA, SunP (2010) Forbidden fruit and the prediction of cigarette smoking. Substance Use & Misuse 45: 1683–1693.
74. Wenger E (1998) Communities of practice: learning, meaning and identity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
75. KrimskyS (2005) The weight of scientific evidence in policy and law. American Journal of Public Health 95: S129–S136.
76. National Cancer institute (2008) The role of the media in promoting and reducing tobacco use. Tobacco Control Monograph Series No 19. Available: http://cancercontrol.cancer.gov/brp/tcrb/monographs/19/m19_complete.pdf.
77. FriedenTR (2010) A framework for public health action: the health impact pyramid. American Journal of Public Health 100: 590–595.
78. Department of Health (2012) Consultation on standardised packaging of tobacco products. London: Department of Health.
79. Imperial Tobacco (2006). Available: https://www.imperial-tobacco.com/files/financial/presentation/011206/ubs_transcript.pdf. Accessed 27 February 2014.
80. WakefieldM, MorleyC, HoranJK, CummingsKM (2002) The cigarette pack as image: new evidence from tobacco industry documents. Tobacco Control 11: I73–I80.
81. CummingsKM, MorleyCP, HoranJK, StegerC, LeavellNR (2002) Marketing to America's youth: evidence from corporate documents. Tobacco Control 11: I5–I17.
82. KotnowskiK, HammondD (2013) The impact of cigarette pack shape, size and opening: evidence from tobacco company documents. Addiction 108: 1658–1668.
83. Proctor R, Schiebinger L (2008) Agnotology - the making and unmaking of ignorance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
84. TobaccoTactics.org. PMI's Anti-PP Media Campaign. Slide 37. Available: http://www.tobaccotactics.org/index.php/PMI%E2%80%99s_Anti-PP_Media_Campaign. Accessed 06 March 2014.
85. TobaccoTactics.org. PMI's Anti-Plain Packaging Lobbying Campaign. Slide 52. Available: http://tobaccotactics.org/index.php/PMI%27s_Anti-Plain_Packaging_Lobbying_Campaign. Accessed 06 March 2014.
86. NeffRA, GoldmanLR (2005) Regulatory parallels to Daubert: Stakeholder influence, “sound science,” and the delayed adoption of health-protective standards. American Journal of Public Health 95: S81–S91.
87. GodleeF, MaloneR, TimmisA, OttoC, BushA, et al. (2013) Journal policy on research funded by the tobacco industry. BMJ 347: f5193.
88. McGarity T, Wagner WE (2008) Bending science: how special interests corrupt public health research. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
89. Latour B (1987) Science in action: how to follow scientists and engineers through society. Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press.
90. JasanoffS (2008) Representation and re-presentation in litigation science. Environmental Health Perspectives 116: 123–129.
91. de CamargoKRJr (2012) How to identify science being bent: The tobacco industry's fight to deny second-hand smoking health hazards as an example. Social Science & Medicine 75: 1230–1235.
92. JasanoffS (1996) Research subpoenas and the sociology of knowledge. Law and Contemporary Problems 59: 95–118.
93. House of Commons Science and Technology Committee (2006) Scientific advice, risk and evidence based policy making. Seventh Report of Session 2005–6 Volume 1. London: The Stationery Office Ltd.
Štítky
Interní lékařstvíČlánek vyšel v časopise
PLOS Medicine
2014 Číslo 3
- Příznivý vliv Armolipidu Plus na hladinu cholesterolu a zánětlivé parametry u pacientů s chronickým subklinickým zánětem
- Léčba bolesti u seniorů
- Co lze v terapii hypertenze očekávat od přidání perindoprilu k bisoprololu?
- Nefarmakologická léčba dyslipidémií
- Flexofytol® – přírodní revoluce v boji proti osteoartróze kloubů
Nejčtenější v tomto čísle
- and Water, Sanitation, and Hygiene: A Committed Relationship
- Representation and Misrepresentation of Scientific Evidence in Contemporary Tobacco Regulation: A Review of Tobacco Industry Submissions to the UK Government Consultation on Standardised Packaging
- The Role of Viral Introductions in Sustaining Community-Based HIV Epidemics in Rural Uganda: Evidence from Spatial Clustering, Phylogenetics, and Egocentric Transmission Models
- How Can Journals Respond to Threats of Libel Litigation?