#PAGE_PARAMS# #ADS_HEAD_SCRIPTS# #MICRODATA#

Adult Consequences of Late Adolescent Alcohol Consumption: A Systematic Review of Cohort Studies


Background:
Although important to public policy, there have been no rigorous evidence syntheses of the long-term consequences of late adolescent drinking.

Methods and Findings:
This systematic review summarises evidence from general population cohort studies of drinking between 15–19 years old and any subsequent outcomes aged 20 or greater, with at least 3 years of follow-up study. Fifty-four studies were included, of which 35 were assessed to be vulnerable to bias and/or confounding. The principal findings are: (1) There is consistent evidence that higher alcohol consumption in late adolescence continues into adulthood and is also associated with alcohol problems including dependence; (2) Although a number of studies suggest links to adult physical and mental health and social consequences, existing evidence is of insufficient quality to warrant causal inferences at this stage.

Conclusions:
There is an urgent need for high quality long-term prospective cohort studies in order to better understand the public health burden that is consequent on late adolescent drinking, both in relation to adult drinking and more broadly. Reducing drinking during late adolescence is likely to be important for preventing long-term adverse consequences as well as protecting against more immediate harms.

: Please see later in the article for the Editors' Summary


Published in the journal: . PLoS Med 8(2): e32767. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.1000413
Category: Research Article
doi: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1000413

Summary

Background:
Although important to public policy, there have been no rigorous evidence syntheses of the long-term consequences of late adolescent drinking.

Methods and Findings:
This systematic review summarises evidence from general population cohort studies of drinking between 15–19 years old and any subsequent outcomes aged 20 or greater, with at least 3 years of follow-up study. Fifty-four studies were included, of which 35 were assessed to be vulnerable to bias and/or confounding. The principal findings are: (1) There is consistent evidence that higher alcohol consumption in late adolescence continues into adulthood and is also associated with alcohol problems including dependence; (2) Although a number of studies suggest links to adult physical and mental health and social consequences, existing evidence is of insufficient quality to warrant causal inferences at this stage.

Conclusions:
There is an urgent need for high quality long-term prospective cohort studies in order to better understand the public health burden that is consequent on late adolescent drinking, both in relation to adult drinking and more broadly. Reducing drinking during late adolescence is likely to be important for preventing long-term adverse consequences as well as protecting against more immediate harms.

: Please see later in the article for the Editors' Summary

Introduction

Alcohol is responsible for approximately 4% of the global burden of disease [1]. This burden is higher in high income countries and among men, accounting for 11% of all male deaths in the World Health Organization (WHO) European region in 2004 [1]. There is global concern about drinking trends among young people, particularly in heavy episodic or “binge” drinking. Prominent among policy responses, in the UK and elsewhere, have been attempts to manage antisocial behaviour related to intoxication in public spaces [2]. Much less attention has been given to risks to adult health and well being.

There have been many cohort studies of the longer term harms associated with adolescent drinking. Some studies suggest that individuals “mature out” of late adolescent drinking patterns [3], whilst others identify enduring effects on drinking and broader health and social functioning in adulthood [4]. In the only available meta-analysis of life-course variability, Johnstone and colleagues [3] evaluated stability in drinking frequency and found settled patterns after the age of 30 following earlier marked discontinuity. There has, however, been no systematic review addressing the consequences of late adolescent drinking in adulthood.

If adolescent drinking does not cause later difficulties with which it may be associated, early intervention on and management of the acute consequences of alcohol consumption, such as antisocial behaviour and unintentional injuries [5], may be the most appropriate community safety and public health responses. If causal relationships do exist, however, this approach will not address the cumulative harms produced by alcohol, unless such intervention successfully modifies the long-term relationship with alcohol, which seems unlikely. The obstacles to causal inference are well known, and bias and confounding in particular must be addressed in cohort studies. A systematic review of cohort studies provides the strongest observational study design to evaluate evidence for causal inference [6]. We thus applied this approach to study the consequences of late adolescent drinking.

Methods

Adolescent alcohol involvement and the potential for subsequent harm have been conceptualised and studied in many different ways. We sought therefore to evaluate the possible effects of any behavioural measure of adolescent alcohol consumption on any adult outcome. The durability of any observed effects is an important study theme due to the likely implications for public health.

Search Strategy and Selection Criteria

The data collection process is illustrated in Figure 1 and the PRISMA checklist is included as Text S1. The literature covering 1964 to 2008 inclusive was initially accessed via electronic databases as proposed by Egger and Davey Smith [7]. This start date identified the oldest cohort study included in the study by Johnstone and colleagues [3]. After piloting the following databases were searched: Medline (via both PubMed and MeSH); Web of Knowledge (including ISI Proceedings); Global Health Archive; Cinhal; PsychInfo; Embase; and HMIC. Configured for a PubMed search, the search terms were (1) Adolescen* OR teen* OR young person OR young people OR young adult; (2) Alcohol* OR binge drinking OR drinking culture OR problem drinking OR drinking problem* OR hazardous drinking OR substance [TI]; (3) Adult* [TI] OR cohort OR longitudinal OR prospective OR lifetime [TI]. Initial screening removed studies that were clearly unrelated to this review.

Fig. 1. PRISMA Flowchart.
PRISMA Flowchart.

Citation searching used both backward and forward procedures, with the bibliographies of relevant studies checked and Science Citation Index used for subsequent citations of these papers. Three journals were hand searched: Addiction Abstracts; Addiction; and Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. The latter two have been published continuously during 1964 to 2008 and were selected following piloting. A data collection protocol was developed and the entire process was undertaken twice, on the second occasion by a research assistant blinded to the outcome of the first. All subsequent study tasks were also duplicated. Only peer-reviewed published data were used and further unpublished information was not sought from authors. Finally, experts, including the authors of included primary studies, were contacted to identify additional studies that had been missed.

The following selection criteria were applied independently by two researchers. Studies of drinking behaviour were included if they collected data on at least two points in time, were at least 3 y apart, and from the same cohort. Data collection regarding alcohol consumption was required between the ages of 15 and 19 y old (or between 9th grade at school or first year of university if age not specified). Drinking is normative in this age group and approaches peak levels towards the end of this age range and into the early 20s in most high income countries [8]. Studies were also required to include a report of at least one quantitative measure of effect, such as an odds ratio (OR), between alcohol involvement and any later outcome assessed at age 20 or greater. Cohorts formed from general population sources, including college students and military conscripts, were included. Studies based on selected or special populations such as children of alcoholics, mental health patients, and offenders were excluded.

Data Analysis

Quality appraisal of included studies was undertaken to evaluate the potential for bias and the adequacy of control for confounding. We gave particular attention to socioeconomic deprivation and other early life sources of vulnerability, as well as indications of other adolescent behavioural problems, in assessing confounding. We designated studies as having stronger capacity for casual inference in relation to the aims of this review if residual confounding in these areas was assessed as unlikely to be important by two reviewers and they had at least one of the following characteristics: (1) follow-up rates of 80% or greater; or (2) sample sizes of 1,000 participants or more. These characteristics identify two forms of bias at the individual study (attrition bias) and at the review level (small studies having disproportionate influence in reviews), respectively. Also if a study had both these characteristics, it was deemed to have stronger capacity for causal inference if both reviewers agreed there was reasonable control of confounding even though residual confounding was nevertheless still likely. We considered that adjustment for some factors while leaving major individual psychosocial confounders uncontrolled, did not constitute reasonable control of confounding. This subset of studies with stronger capacity for causal inference is individually discussed in the narrative presentation of results in order to summarise the evidence base. Two researchers agreed on all bar two studies (Kappa 0.91) [9],[10], for which disagreements about the strength of control for confounding were resolved by discussion. Meta-analysis of pooled outcomes from these observational studies was deemed inappropriate only after consideration of the nature of findings from included studies, because of the potentially misleading nature of such summary effect estimates in the context of uncontrolled bias and confounding [7].

Results

The majority of included studies (n = 35) were multiple reports from ten cohorts (see Table 1), with the Swedish Conscript Study (SCS) of male conscripts providing nine separate reports [11][19]. The remainder of included studies (n = 19) originated from separate cohorts (see Table 2). Tables 1 and 2 present selected study characteristics including variables involved in eligible measures of effect. Altogether a total of 54 studies were eligible for inclusion in this review [9][62]. This literature has grown rapidly in recent years, with approximately two-thirds of studies (n = 35) published since 2001. Approximately half of all reports (n = 26) were from US studies, ten were from Sweden, eight from Britain, four from New Zealand, three from Australia, two from Finland, and one from the Netherlands. More than half (n = 30) originated from school-based cohorts. Birth cohorts were more likely to be the subject of multiple studies (n = 11/14). Nineteen (35%) studies, based on eight different cohorts, were assessed as having stronger capacity for causal inference (see Tables 1 and 2), and we focus primarily on these studies. The presentation of main results is organised by principal outcomes evaluated, with quantitative data presented only from the subset of 19 studies.

Tab. 1. Multiple reports for ten cohort studies.
Multiple reports for ten cohort studies.
AA, alcohol abuse; AD, alcohol dependence; AU, alcohol use; AUD, alcohol use disorder; Beh, behaviour.

Tab. 2. Individual cohort reports.
Individual cohort reports.
AA, alcohol abuse; AD, alcohol dependence; AHRS, Adolescent Health Risk Study; AU, alcohol use; AUD, alcohol use disorder.

Mortality

The risk of premature death associated with late adolescent drinking has been evaluated only in the SCS, after 15, 20, and 25 y [12],[15],[17]. When the male study population was approximately aged 34, late adolescent heavier drinkers (>250 g per week) were twice as likely (OR = 2.1, 95% confidence interval [1.4–3.2]) to have died compared to moderate drinkers (<100 g per week) [12]. This effect was attenuated by age 39 y (OR = 1.46 [1.05–2.04]) at which time potential confounding was more tightly controlled [15]. The majority of deaths at both study intervals were caused by car crashes and suicides. Car crashes were the leading cause of death at younger ages, after which time suicides predominated (see below) [15].

The risk of death among heavier adolescent drinkers due to alcohol-specific causes (International Classification of Diseases 8 [ICD 8] codes 291, 303, and 980 for alcohol psychosis, alcoholism, and alcohol intoxication, respectively) was high (OR = 13.7 [5.3–35.5]) compared to moderate drinkers, as were deaths due to liver cirrhosis and pancreatitis (ICD8 codes 571 and 577; OR = 11.0 [3.2–45.1]), though there were few such cases [15]. Heavier drinkers also differed from others in the ways in which other psychosocial factors impacted upon mortality risk. Among all conscripts high psychosocial risk (defined as 5 or more risk factors, compared to 0–2 factors) was associated with a 3-fold elevation in observed mortality (OR = 3.0 [2.3–4.0]). Among heavier drinkers there was no independent effect of psychosocial risk factor groups on mortality (OR = 1.3 [0.7–2.7]), meaning that “good social adjustment as indicated by absence of other risk factors constitutes little or no protection from an increased risk of premature mortality among high consumers of alcohol” [15].

Alcohol consumption was categorised slightly differently in the 25-y follow-up, complicating direct comparisons with earlier data [17]. Those drinking 15 g per day or greater were at heightened risk of early mortality (OR = 1.37 [1.01–1.85]) in comparison with a reference group of abstainers, with the risk slightly more pronounced among those drinking 30 g per day or more (OR = 1.53 [1.08–2.16]). Alcohol was estimated to have caused 14% of all deaths. Neither trends in protective effects on myocardial infarction hospitalisation or death nor on risk of stroke approached statistical significance [17].

Alcohol Consumption

More than 20 studies provided evidence of associations between late adolescent alcohol consumption and subsequent drinking in adulthood, with one study reporting no associations based on a limited measure of alcohol involvement (see Tables 1 and 2 and below) [33]. There were five studies with stronger capacity for causal inference, four of which were published since 2004.

In the New Zealand birth cohort Christchurch Health and Development Study (CHDS), effects of an age 16 latent class variable on all drinking frequency and quantity outcomes at age 21 survived extensive adjustment for covariates. These controls eliminated apparent relationships between age 16 drinking and most other outcomes [60]. Latent classes were formed by consumption and frequency measures and were most importantly influenced by the largest amount consumed on a single occasion in the past 3 mo. In the British birth cohort National Child Development Study (NCDS) effects of overall weekly consumption at age 16 on this same measure were observed until age 23 y, as far as was studied [38].

Kandel and colleagues [33] found no direct relationship between having drunk alcohol ten times ever by age 16 and alcohol consumption behaviours at age 24–25 y in the New York Longitudinal Study (NYLS) cohort representative of school attenders in that state. Bogart and colleagues [9] found a small effect of drinking frequency measured using a 12-point scale at age 18 on whether any alcohol was being drunk (OR = 1.08 [1.01–1.15]) and on heavy episodic drinking (OR = 1.11 [1.01–1.22]), with no effect on overall monthly consumption among 29-y-old women in a subgroup analysis of the RAND cohort.

Both monthly frequency of consumption and heavy episodic drinking at age 18 were related to the same measures at 22, 26, and 35 y in the nationally representative US Monitoring the Future Cohort Study (MFCS) [43]. The MFCS identified these effects to be much larger than all other adolescent sociodemographic, parental, psychological, and behavioural predictors of both drinking frequency and heavy drinking outcomes. Standardised regression coefficients for both variables were on average at least twice as large as any other predictor across all follow-up intervals. These effects diminished over time, for example being 0.34, 0.21, and 0.18 for drinking frequency at the three ages (all p<0.001). They were also stronger for males than females in heavy drinking (regression coefficients approximately three times larger than any other), whereas there was no evidence of gender difference in monthly consumption frequency [43].

Alcohol Problems Including Dependence

All studies assessing alcohol problems or dependence in adulthood found statistically significant associations with late adolescent drinking. Among seven studies with stronger capacity for causal inference, three were drawn from the SCS and two from the CHDS cohorts.

In CHDS, effects of age 16 alcohol consumption latent class on Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV) alcohol dependence at age 21, were larger than on any other alcohol-related outcome [60]. Diagnosis of alcohol abuse at age 18 was related to later alcohol diagnoses at 21 y (abuse OR = 2.6 [1.7–4.1], dependence OR = 3.0 [1.2–7.2]) [61]. Diagnosis of dependence at age 18 was associated with age 21 diagnosis of abuse at 21 (OR = 3.5 [1.8–6.7]) and more strongly with dependence (OR = 15.5 [6.0–40.1]) after good control for confounding in this birth cohort [61].

Similar to the findings on alcohol consumption, the RAND female study [9] found a small effect of age 18 drinking frequency measured on a 12-point scale on negative consequences attributed to alcohol in the past year at age 29 (OR = 1.12 [1.04–1.21]). There was no effect on the number of these consequences. As with their previous findings, these effects were smaller than the protective effects of marriage on women's drinking behaviour at 29. Longer term consequences to age 35 were again apparent in MFCS, with heavy episodic drinking at 18 being predictive of both DSM-IV abuse and dependence (standardised regression coefficients 0.09 [p<0.01] and 0.08 [p<0.001], respectively). Unlike effects on alcohol consumption, however, the effects of adolescent heavy drinking on alcohol problems at 35 were no longer larger than other possible component causes including parental drinking, theft/property damage, marijuana, and other illicit drug use at age 18 (regression coefficients broadly similar to heavy drinking and statistically significant) [43].

At approximately age 34, heavy drinking (>250 g per week) young men at age 19 were 2.3 (1.8–2.9) times more likely to have been hospitalised for alcoholism than low risk drinkers (<100 g per week) in the SCS [16]. Elevated risk also in the intermediate category provided evidence of a dose response relationship (OR = 1.6 [1.3–1.9]). Among other predictors, the relative risk was greater only among those having had earlier contact with police or child care authorities [16]. In the Stockholm Country subset of the SCS heavier drinking at conscription was not associated with public drunkenness offences 7–8 y later (approximate age 26–27 y) after adjustment for confounders [19]. After 26–27 y of follow-up of the national cohort (approximate age 45–46 y), a summary measure of problematic drinking at conscription was not associated with hospitalization or mortality with an alcohol diagnosis (OR = 1.33 [0.89–1.99] in the subgroup who had not used cannabis) [18].

Car Crashes and Drink Driving Offences

SCS mortality data have already been presented. Among Stockholm County conscripts, heavier alcohol consumption was not associated with drinking and driving offences after 7–8 y with adjustment for confounders [19]. The relative risk of car crash fatality in the entire SCS, however, was 2.3 (1.2–4.3) for heavier drinkers (>250 g per week) compared to moderate drinkers and 8.0 (2.2–28.9) compared to abstainers after 20 y of follow-up [15]. Moderate drinkers (<100 g per week), were also at elevated risk compared to abstainers (OR 3.5 [1.1–10.7]). CHDS identified a possible effect on any drink driving offences by age 21 of borderline statistical significance attributable to age 16 drinking pattern and no effect on speeding [60]. Shope and colleagues [53] found that the possible effects of a combined measure of alcohol use and problems assessed at age 15 before driving began on the numbers of serious crashes and driving offences to ages 23–24 were not robust to confounding in a US school cohort.

Other Criminal Convictions

The CHDS found no effect of age 16 drinking latent class on court convictions or property offences by age 21 [60]. Effects on numbers of violent offences were, however, identified to be robust to adjustment for background variables. In the Stockholm County segment of the SCS a dichotomised adolescent alcohol problems measure was predictive of having any officially recorded criminal convictions during 15 y of follow-up (OR = 1.31, confidence interval not provided, p<0.001) [11].

Mental Health

There were no age 16 alcohol effects on any of the mental health outcomes (major depression, anxiety disorder, suicidal ideation, and suicide attempt, all p>0.5 after adjustment) at age 21 assessed in CHDS [60]. There were no associations between age 15–16 drinking lifetime prevalence and having seen a mental health professional, nor on depressed mood at age 24–25 in the NYLS [33].

As noted above, suicide was the leading cause of death over the 20 y of the SCS mortality studies, with heavier drinkers at greater risk than moderate drinkers (OR = 1.7 [1.0–2.8]) and abstainers [15]. Other risk factors were more strongly associated with suicide, including number of friends (having none compared to having more than 3, OR = 3.1 [1.5–6.2]). Although there was also a greater risk of psychiatric hospitalisation after 15 y in the SCS (OR 1.8 [1.5–2.1]), the nonaddiction mental health consequences are difficult to appreciate as approximately two-thirds of all admissions involved alcoholism or drug addiction [14].

Tobacco Smoking

An observed association between age 16 drinking patterns and DSM-IV nicotine dependence at age 21 disappeared after adjustment for covariates in CHDS [60]. In the US Adolescent Health Risk Study (AHRS) [31] small effects of occasional and heavy drinking on smoking initiation and cessation were identified over a 5-y interval into the early 20s. ORs for both regular smoking and half pack a day smoking suggested small effects, with lower confidence intervals near 1. These were statistically significant for all initiation analyses (among nonsmokers) but not in all cessation analyses (among baseline smokers). No effects of having drunk alcohol 10 times or more by age 15–16 on lifetime smoking prevalence by 24–25 y were observed in the NYLS [33].

Other Drug Use and Related Problems

In CHDS age 16 drinking latent class was not associated with age 21 cannabis and other illicit drug dependence after control for confounding [60]. No effects of age 15–16 drinking lifetime prevalence on other drug use at 24–25 were observed in the NYLS [33]. Despite this cohort being particularly closely associated with the “gateway” perspective (any prior use of one drug increasing risk of subsequent use of another), only cumulative use measured over the entire intervening period was associated with other drug use, thus “adolescent use retains no direct unique effect once use between adolescence and young adulthood is taken into account” [33]. In the SCS after 26–27 y of follow-up, a summary measure of problematic drinking among those who had not used cannabis at conscription was not associated with hospitalization or mortality with a drug use diagnosis (OR = 1.83 [0.97–3.45]) [18]. Among problem drinkers who had used cannabis, however, elevated risks of such drug problems were found (ever used ≤10 times OR = 5.60 [2.92–10.75], >10 times 3.34 [1.60–6.98]).

Educational Attainment

No effects of adolescent drinking on any educational outcomes (school qualification, university enrolment, or degree) at age 21 remained after adjustment for confounding in CHDS [60]. In the NYLS there was no effect of having drunk alcohol ten times or more by age 15–16 on number of years in education by 24–25 y [33]. In the British NCDS, however, an effect of heavier past week drinking (>4 units male, >3 units female) at age 16 on subsequent educational attainment by age 42 was found among men only, using propensity scores to deal with confounding [54]. This effect was greater in working class men, where heavy drinkers were approximately 25% less likely to complete a degree than nonheavy drinkers. The difference was 10% among middle class men [54].

Other Possible Consequences

The CHDS found no effect of age 16 drinking latent class on months unemployed, sexually transmitted infections, or pregnancy by age 21 [60]. Effects on numbers of sexual partners were identified to be robust to adjustment for background variables. Hospitalisation during 15 y of follow-up for any accidents, or gastro-intestinal, respiratory, musculo-skeletal, or infectious disorders in Stockholm County in the SCS was not significantly higher (OR 1.2 [0.9–1.6]) for heavier drinkers as compared to moderate drinkers as defined above [13]. The NYLS found no direct effects on a wide range of possible consequences of having ever drank alcohol ten times or more by age 15–16. Given the limitations of this measure it is perhaps not surprising that alcohol consequences, compared to other substances, were characterised as “benign” [33]. Finally, there were no effects of drinking frequency at age 18 on life satisfaction at age 29 in a western US school cohort [10], again in contrast to other substances.

Additional Evidence from Other Studies

Evidence from studies not assessed as having stronger capacity for inference broadly agreed with the findings presented above. There was consistent evidence of effects on subsequent alcohol consumption and related problems. There were similarly mixed findings on possible mental health, tobacco smoking, and educational consequences. There are two principal exceptions: Although no effects on pregnancy among young women were found in CHDS, contrary findings emerged from two other cohorts [46],[59], one of which was restricted to pregnancy outcome by age 18 [59]. Also in three of the four studies that investigated possible effects on adult drug use or related problems, associations with at least one outcome measure in this area were identified [40],[46],[59]. In both cases, the vulnerability of these findings to bias and/or confounding should be remembered.

Discussion

This systematic review has investigated whether late adolescent alcohol consumption is a time-limited activity without significant longer term consequences or whether it impacts upon adult health and well being. It is clear that the evidence base on long-term consequences is not as extensive nor as compelling as it could be. There are sparse data of sufficient quality to warrant making causal inferences on the broader health and social consequences of late adolescent drinking on the basis of the data evaluated here.

There is evidence from a single population-based cohort that late adolescent drinking can cause early death among men, principally through car crashes and suicides [12],[15]. There is a large evidence base attesting to the ongoing impacts of late adolescent drinking on adult drinking behaviours, though most studies cannot strongly support causal inferences because of their designs. There is robust evidence from one national cohort that apparent effects on later alcohol consumption persist beyond the age of 30, which is longer than had previously been understood [43]. Possible effects on subsequent alcohol problems including dependence are somewhat more complex than effects upon subsequent alcohol consumption per se. Evidence from multiple well-designed cohort studies indicates that other factors indicative of heightened psychosocial risk more broadly are also implicated. It is nonetheless striking that effects on alcohol problems assessed in the mid 30s appear to have been produced by elevated consumption in late adolescence in both SCS and MFCS, and to earlier ages in other studies. Findings from a rigorous birth cohort study on nonalcohol outcomes, however, demonstrate that many apparent effects of late adolescent drinking may be due to uncontrolled confounding [60]. Certainty about the long-term consequences of late adolescent drinking is thus not easily achieved.

Caution is also required because of the limitations of the present study. Our approach to the investigation of confounding in individual studies was necessarily constrained by weaknesses of the literature as a whole and by the comprehensive nature of our appraisal of possible consequences. For example, there are few studies that address family influences, both siblings and parents. Similarly, there was only a single study included that investigated genetic inheritance [58]. Confounding also needs to be considered in relation to more specific outcomes. For example, a CHDS study not included here by virtue of not having a drinking behavioural measure, found that the contribution of drink driving to traffic accidents is much reduced when other risky driver behaviours are taken into account [63]. Unless otherwise indicated in the Results section, traditional methods of investigation of confounding have been used in the studies covered and these can be criticised for their adequacy in dealing with residual confounding. Being designated as having stronger capacity for casual inference here should not be mistaken to indicate support for causal inference in relation to observed associations, nor that we view the treatment of psychosocial factors to have eliminated residual confounding. Conversely, not being so designated does not imply that a study is weak, rather, simply that it does not contribute as much as others to the aims of this review.

Bias was considered in relation to sample size and study attrition in our appraisal of study quality. Whilst this latter variable is long established as important to the evaluation of cohort studies, thinking about the former has been more recently advanced, and thus deserves elaboration and consideration of the impact on review findings. Small study effects have been recently observed within meta-analyses of trials [64]. These effects are related to publication bias (see below) though are more fundamentally due to the greater likelihood of bias in effect estimates in small studies compared to large studies [65]. We are not aware that the influence of small study effects has previously been considered within systematic reviews of cohort studies despite their potential for bias. At the outset, we judged that it was important to consider this possibility, and to use a simple means of so doing, given the challenges involved in summarising a large number of observational studies with substantial problems of bias and confounding. We took the decision to give additional weight to larger studies after piloting and before the main study data collection and analysis. The particular threshold we chose may be somewhat arbitrary, though it is not clear that moving this threshold upwards or downwards by a few hundred study participants would substantially influence our findings.

Two other important forms of bias were not directly involved in the determination of study quality. Almost all adolescent behavioural data were self-reported. These data are most likely to involve underestimation of true levels of drinking and its consequences for reasons of social desirability, though the possibility of exaggeration should also not be ignored [66],[67]. Self-report bias leads to underestimation of the true extent of the relationships between adolescent exposure and adult consequences, as would also be true if reporting error was random rather than systematic. This problem is compounded by the fact that the vast majority of the adult outcome data are also self-reported, making probable further underestimation of the true effects [68], notwithstanding the effects of repeated measures within cohorts. The SCS studies are a noteworthy exception to the reliance on self-reported outcomes. It would have been possible to have selected data reliability as a criterion for bias evaluation; this would not have meaningfully changed the results beyond giving greater prominence to SCS data.

Publication bias exerts influence in the opposite direction [69],[70]. If the studies reviewed here represent a biased sample of all the relevant studies that have been undertaken, then overestimation of actual effects occurs, which seems highly likely given the paucity of negative findings for alcohol outcomes. This threat to valid inference perhaps has its origins in the context of preliminary explorations of cohort study datasets. In these situations, if drinking is not found to be associated with outcomes of interest then the analyses may not be pursued. We may also have missed studies that meet inclusion criteria by virtue of their publication characteristics. This risk is inherent in the nature of this exercise and is heightened given the breadth of the outcomes investigated here. It seems likely, therefore, that the possibility of not having successfully identified all relevant studies is greater for nonalcohol compared to alcohol outcomes. At the outset we expected publication bias to be a greater threat to the validity of inferences made than small study effects. By its nature, however, we were not in a position to attribute this risk to individual included studies. Both publication and reporting bias pose profound threats to valid inference in this review whose magnitude is difficult to appreciate quantitatively.

Studies from a range of different national and cultural contexts have been included here and identified as providing a stronger basis for causal attribution, though these are entirely restricted to Anglophone and Northern European countries. There are no included studies from low- and middle-income countries, nor from any country with a Mediterranean drinking culture. Previous meta-analytic study in this area has identified national context to be particularly important to findings on alcohol consumption from cohort studies [3]. These studies also cover limited historical periods. Period effects were investigated across the different cohorts in the MFCS where only limited differences were identified [43].

Because there have been no previous systematic reviews of this literature, the research question addressed here is unusually broad. This approach led us to specify an end date for formal inclusion in the review, and inevitably further studies have since been published. For example, Huurre and colleagues [71] robustly identified continuities between heavy drinking at age 16 and hazardous drinking at age 32 in a Finnish study. A later MFCS report demonstrates the application of multilevel analyses to examine more advanced research questions on mediators and moderators of effects [72]. Other more recent studies that may have been included [73],[74] do not substantially change the picture obtained, though there will be other studies of which we are unaware and we expect this literature to continue to grow rapidly.

Notwithstanding the limitations of the evidence base and of this review, and attenuations over time in the strength of the direct effects, late adolescent alcohol consumption appears a probable cause of increased drinking well into adulthood, through to ages at which adult social roles have been achieved. Heavier drinking seems most likely, however, to be only one component in a complex causal process, whose contribution has probably been overestimated in previous studies because of uncontrolled confounding, setting aside the uncertainties induced by self-reported data. The importance of these data is highlighted in the context of work showing strong stability of drinking patterns through the fourth and fifth decades of life [3],[31],[75]. A wide range of health and other harms, such as liver cirrhosis, are caused by alcohol at middle and older ages [76],[77]. Late adolescent drinking, by virtue of its probable effect on long-term adult alcohol consumption is likely to contribute to the burden of alcohol-related disease. Continuities from adolescence to adulthood in drinking patterns have been observed across a range of measures including frequency of consumption and heavy drinking.

In this study it seems that alcohol consumption confers additional risk of alcohol problems both on those who are already more vulnerable in various ways to poorer health and psychosocial outcomes, and strikingly also among those who are not otherwise vulnerable. Possible effects on adult alcohol problems and dependence including hospitalisation identified here result from heavier drinking in adolescence without necessarily involving problems at younger ages. If these effects are confirmed, there are two important implications: (1) Reducing late adolescent alcohol consumption in the general population may be expected to make a long-term contribution to reducing the incidence of adult alcohol problems; (2) In more vulnerable populations, late adolescent drinking may be one cause among many of later difficulties, and its effects may be more severe and long-lasting [78]. Having relatively secure psychosocial resources may somewhat buffer these risks, and their consequent potential for adverse effects, but it does not remove them. These statements should be read with some caution given studies of mediators and moderators of these effects are lacking, limiting our understanding of their nature. Nevertheless, this systematic review affords more secure inference of the likely existence of these effects than has been possible previously. It is possible that relationships with alcohol forged during late adolescence may have cumulative lifetime drinking related consequences that are also simply not well captured by the existing literature.

The lack of convincing evidence of effects on nonalcohol outcomes is the product of an absence of evidence rather than strong evidence indicating no effects. A priori, one might expect that effects on nonalcohol outcomes would be weaker simply because they are less direct. To this extent, any such effects would be less durable and may be more likely to occur in high risk subgroups. There is also the possibility of reverse causation in relation to many of these consequences as the initiation of drinking in adolescence may have been preceded by many of the nonalcohol outcomes considered here. Only careful studies of adolescence may address this possibility.

There is also a clear need for high quality long-term prospective cohort studies in order to better understand the public health burden that is consequent on late adolescent drinking, both in relation to adult drinking and more broadly. A number of the cohorts included here were originally formed for prevention trials, and obtained some short-term evidence of benefit (for example the RAND and Seattle cohorts in Table 1). There is currently, however, an absence of experimental evidence of successful intervention modifying drinking during the late adolescent years leading to improved adult outcomes [79]. Long-term investment in rectifying this state of affairs should be a public health priority.

In addition to making both alcohol and heavy drinking less available, less acceptable, and more expensive [80],[81], these findings indicate a need for policy makers to encourage young people to be more cognisant of the long-term risks to adult health and well-being, and to act on this awareness in their decision making about whether and how much to drink [82]. This encouragement requires much more than the provision of accurate information about risks if it is to have any real prospect of influencing actual behaviour. Alcohol harm reduction has largely been concerned with reducing various risks inherent in drinking situations and their immediate aftermaths [81]. This study demonstrates the need to develop a longer term perspective on harm reduction.

Supporting Information

Attachment 1


Zdroje

1. RehmJ

MathersC

PopovaS

ThavorncharoensapM

TeerawattananonY

2009

Global burden of disease and injury and economic cost attributable to alcohol use and alcohol-use disorders.

Lancet

373

2223

2233

2. Department of Health

2007

Safe. Sensible. Social. The next steps in the National Alcohol Strategy-a summary.

London

Alcohol Policy Team

3. JohnstoneBM

LeinoEV

AgerCR

FerrerH

Middleton-FillmoreK

1996

Determinants of life-course variation in the frequency of alcohol consumption: Meta-analysis of studies from the collaborative alcohol-related longitudinal project.

J Stud Alcohol

57

494

506

4. McCartyCA

EbelBE

GarrisonMM

Di GiuseppeDL

ChristakisDA

2004

Continuity of binge and harmful drinking from late adolescence to early adulthood.

Pediatrics

114

714

719

5. PattonGC

CoffeyC

SawyerSM

VinerRM

HallerDM

2009

Global patterns of mortality in young people: a systematic analysis of population health data.

Lancet

374

881

892

6. Oxford Centre for Evidence-based Medicine

2001

Levels of medicine.

Oxford

Oxford Centre for evidence-based medicine

7. EggerM

Davey SmithG

2001

Principles of and procedures for systematic review.

EggerM

Davey SmithG

AltmanDG

Systematic reviews in health care: meta-analysis in context. 2nd edition

London

BMJ Publishing Group

8. BrownSA

McGueM

MaggsJ

SchulenbergJ

HingsonR

2008

A developmental perspective on alcohol and youths 16 to 20 years of age.

Pediatrics

121

Suppl 4

S290

S310

9. BogartLM

CollinsRL

EllicksonPL

MartinoSC

KleinDJ

2005

Effects of early and later marriage on women's alcohol use in young adulthood: a prospective analysis.

J Stud Alcohol Drugs

66

729

737

10. BogartLM

CollinsRL

EllicksonPL

KleinDJ

2007

Are adolescent substance users less satisfied with life as young adults and if so, why?

Soc Indic Res

81

149

169

11. StattinH

Romelsjo, A.

1995

Adult mortality in the light of criminality, substance abuse, and behavioural and family risk factors in adolescence.

Crim Behav Ment Health

5

279

311

12. AndreassonS

AllebeckP

RomelsjoA

1988

Alcohol and mortality among young men: longitudinal study of Swedish conscripts.

Br Med J (Clin Res Ed)

296

1021

1025

13. AndreassonS

AllebeckP

RomelsjoA

1990

Hospital admissions for somatic care among young men: the role of alcohol.

Br J Addict

85

935

941

14. AndreassonS

AllebeckP

1991

Alcohol and psychiatric illness: longitudinal study of psychiatric admissions in a cohort of Swedish conscripts.

Subst Use Misuse

26

713

728

15. AndreassonS

RomelsjoA

AllebeckP

1991

Alcohol, social factors and mortality among young men.

Br J Addict

86

877

887

16. AndreassonS

AllebeckP

BrandtL

1993

Predictors of alcoholism in young Swedish men.

Am J Public Health

83

845

850

17. RomelsjoA

LeifmanA

1999

Association between alcohol consumption and mortality, myocardial infarction, and stroke in 25 year follow up of 49 618 young Swedish men.

BMJ

319

821

822

18. StenbackaM

2003

Problematic alcohol and cannabis use in adolescence—risk of serious adult substance abuse?

Drug Alcohol Rev

22

277

286

19. KarlssonG

RomelsjoA

1997

A longitudinal study of social, psychological and behavioural factors associated with drunken driving and public drunkenness.

Addiction

92

447

457

20. BinghamCR

ShopeJT

TangX

2005

Drinking behavior from high school to young adulthood: differences by college education.

Alcohol Clin Exp Res

29

2170

2180

21. BonomoYA

BowesG

CoffeyC

CarlinJB

PattonGC

2004

Teenage drinking and the onset of alcohol dependence: a cohort study over seven years.

Addiction

99

1520

1528

22. CableN

SackerA

2008

Typologies of alcohol consumption in adolescence: predictors and adult outcomes.

Alcohol Alcohol

43

81

90

23. CasswellS

ZhangJF

1997

Access to alcohol from licensed premises during adolescence: a longitudinal study.

Addiction

92

737

745

24. ChatterjiP

2006

Does alcohol use during high school affect educational attainment? Evidence from the National Education Longitudinal Study.

Econ Educ Rev

25

482

497

25. CollinsRL

EllicksonPL

KleinDJ

2007

The role of substance use in young adult divorce.

Addiction

102

786

794

26. D'AmicoEJ

EllicksonPL

CollinsRL

MartinoS

KleinDJ

2005

Processes linking adolescent problems to substance-use problems in late young adulthood.

J Stud Alcohol Drugs

66

766

775

27. DonovanJE

JessorR

JessorL

1983

Problem drinking in adolescence and young adulthood. A follow-up study.

J Stud Alcohol Drugs

44

109

137

28. EnglundMM

EgelandB

OlivaEM

CollinsWA

2008

Childhood and adolescent predictors of heavy drinking and alcohol use disorders in early adulthood: a longitudinal developmental analysis. (Special Issue: Destiny matters: childhood and adolescent prediction of adult alcohol use and abuse in six multi-decade longitudinal studies.).

Addiction

103

23

35

29. GhodsianM

PowerC

1987

Alcohol consumption between the ages of 16 and 23 in Britain: a longitudinal study.

Br J Addict

82

175

180

30. GuoJ

HawkinsJ

HillKG

AbbottRD

2001

Childhood and adolescent predictors of alcohol abuse and dependence in young adulthood.

J Stud Alcohol Drugs

62

754

762

31. JacksonKM

SherKJ

CooperM

WoodPK

2002

Adolescent alcohol and tobacco use: onset, persistence and trajectories of use across two samples.

Addiction

97

517

531

32. JefferisB

PowerC

ManorO

2005

Adolescent drinking level and adult binge drinking in a national birth cohort.

Addiction

100

543

549

33. KandelDB

DaviesM

KarusD

YamaguchiK

1986

The consequences in young adulthood of adolescent drug involvement. An overview.

Arch Gen Psychiatry

43

746

754

35. KoppesLL

KemperHC

PostGB

SnelJ

TwiskJW

2000

Development and stability of alcohol consumption from adolescence into adulthood: the Amsterdam Growth and Health Longitudinal Study.

Eur Addict Res

6

183

188

36. LockeTF

NewcombMD

2001

Alcohol involvement and dysphoria: a longitudinal examination of gender differences from late adolescence to adulthood.

Psychol Addict Behav

15

227

236

36. LockeT

NewcombMD

2003

Psychosocial outcomes of alcohol involvement and dysphoria in women: a 16-year prospective community study.

J Stud Alcohol Drugs

64

531

546

37. LockeTF

NewcombMD

2004

Adolescent predictors of young adult and adult alcohol involvement and dysphoria in a prospective community sample of women.

Prev Sci

5

151

168

38. MaggsJL

PatrickME

FeinsteinL

2008

Childhood and adolescent predictors of alcohol use and problems in adolescence and adulthood in the National Child Development Study.

Addiction

103

Suppl 1

7

22

39. MasonW

KostermanR

HaggertyKP

HawkinsJ

RedmondC

2008

Dimensions of adolescent alcohol involvement as predictors of young-adult major depression.

J Stud Alcohol Drugs

69

275

285

40. McGeeR

WilliamsS

PoultonR

MoffittT

2000

A longitudinal study of cannabis use and mental health from adolescence to early adulthood.

Addiction

95

491

503

41. McMorrisBJ

UggenC

2000

Alcohol and employment in the transition to adulthood.

J Health Soc Behav

41

276

294

42. MerlineAC

O'MalleyPM

SchulenbergJE

BachmanJG

JohnstonLD

2004

Substance use among adults 35 years of age: prevalence, adulthood predictors, and impact of adolescent substance use.

Am J Public Health

94

96

102

43. MerlineA

JagerJ

SchulenbergJE

2008

Adolescent risk factors for adult alcohol use and abuse: stability and change of predictive value across early and middle adulthood.

Addiction

103

84

99

44. OesterleS

HillKG

HawkinsJD

AbbottRD

2008

Positive functioning and alcohol-use disorders from adolescence to young adulthood.

J Stud Alcohol Drugs

69

100

111

45. PaavolaM

VartiainenE

HaukkalaA

2004

Smoking, alcohol use, and physical activity: a 13-year longitudinal study ranging from adolescence into adulthood.

J Adolesc Health

35

238

244

46. PattonGC

CoffeyC

LynskeyMT

ReidS

HemphillS

2007

Trajectories of adolescent alcohol and cannabis use into young adulthood.

Addiction

102

607

615

47. PeckSC

VidaM

EcclesJS

2008

Adolescent pathways to adulthood drinking: sport activity involvement is not necessarily risky or protective.

Addiction

103

69

83

48. PowerC

EstaughV

1990

Employment and drinking in early adulthood: a longitudinal perspective.

Br J Addict

85

487

494

49. RepettoPB

ZimmermanMA

CaldwellCH

2004

A longitudinal study of the relationship between depressive symptoms and alcohol use in a sample of inner-city Black youth.

J Stud Alcohol Drugs

65

169

178

50. RohdeP

LewinsohnPM

KahlerCW

SeeleyJR

BrownRA

2001

Natural course of alcohol use disorders from adolescence to young adulthood.

J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry

40

83

90

51. SchulenbergJ

WadsworthKN

OmalleyPM

BachmanJG

JohnstonLD

1996

Adolescent risk factors for binge drinking during the transition to young adulthood: variable- and pattern-centered approaches to change.

Dev Psychol

32

659

674

52. ShepherdJ

FarringtonD

PottsJ

2004

Impact of antisocial lifestyle on health.

J Public Health (Oxf)

26

347

352

53. ShopeJT

WallerPF

RaghunathanTE

PatilSM

2001

Adolescent antecedents of high-risk driving behavior into young adulthood: substance use and parental influences.

Accid Anal Prev

33

649

658

54. StaffJ

PatrickME

LokenE

MaggsJL

2008

Teenage alcohol use and educational attainment.

J Stud Alcohol Drugs

69

848

858

55. SteinJA

SmithGM

GuySM

BentlerPM

1993

Consequences of adolescent drug-use on young-adult job behavior and job-satisfaction.

J Appl Psychol

78

463

474

56. ToumbourouJW

WilliamsI

WhiteV

SnowP

MunroG

2004

Prediction of alcohol-related harm from controlled drinking strategies and alcohol consumption trajectories.

Addiction

99

498

508

57. TuckerJS

EllicksonPL

KleinDJ

2003

Predictors of the transition to regular smoking during adolescence and young adulthood.

J Adolesc Health

32

314

324

58. VikenRJ

KaprioJ

RoseRJ

2007

Personality at ages 16 and 17 and drinking problems at ages 18 and 25: genetic analyses of data from Finn Twin16-25.

Twin Res Hum Genet

10

25

32

59. VinerR

TaylorB

2007

Adult outcomes of binge drinking in adolescence: findings from a UK national birth cohort.

J Epidemiol Community Health

61

902

907

60. WellsJE

HorwoodLJ

FergussoDM

2004

Drinking patterns in mid-adolescence and psychosocial outcomes in late adolescence and early adulthood.

Addiction

99

1529

1541

61. WellsJE

HorwoodLJ

FergussonDM

2006

Stability and instability in alcohol diagnosis from ages 18 to 21 and ages 21 to 25 years.

Drug Alcohol Depend

81

157

165

62. WennbergP

AnderssonT

BohmanM

2000

Associations between different aspects of alcohol habits in adolescence, early adulthood, and early middle age: a prospective longitudinal study of a representative cohort of men and women.

Psychol Addict Behav

14

303

307

63. HorwoodLJ

FergussonDM

2000

Drink driving and traffic accidents in young people.

Accid Anal Prev

32

805

814

64. NueschE

TrelleS

ReichenbachS

RutjesAW

TschannenB

2010

Small study effects in meta-analyses of osteoarthritis trials: meta-epidemiological study.

BMJ

341

c3515

65. CoyneJC

ThombsBD

HagedoornM

2010

Ain't necessarily so: review and critique of recent meta-analyses of behavioral medicine interventions in health psychology.

Health Psychol

29

107

116

66. HarrisonL

1995

The validity of self-reported data on drug use.

J Drug Issues

25

91

111

67. TourangeauR

YanT

2007

Sensitive questions in surveys.

Psychol Bull

133

859

883

68. RehmJ

1998

Measuring quantity, frequency, and volume of drinking.

Alcohol Clin Exp Res

22

4S

14S

69. MisakianAL

BeroLA

1998

Publication bias and research on passive smoking: comparison of published and unpublished studies.

JAMA

280

250

253

70. McCambridgeJ

2007

A case study of publication bias in an influential series of reviews of drug education.

Drug Alcohol Rev

26

463

468

71. HuurreT

LintonenT

KaprioJ

PelkonenM

MarttunenM

2010

Adolescent risk factors for excessive alcohol use at age 32 years. A 16-year prospective follow-up study.

Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol

45

125

134

72. StaffJ

SchulenbergJE

MaslowskyJ

BachmanJG

O'MalleyPM

2010

Substance use changes and social role transitions: proximal developmental effects on ongoing trajectories from late adolescence through early adulthood.

Dev Psychopathol

22

917

932

73. MasonWA

HitchJE

KostermanR

McCartyCA

HerrenkohlTI

2010

Growth in adolescent delinquency and alcohol use in relation to young adult crime, alcohol use disorders, and risky sex: a comparison of youth from low- versus middle-income backgrounds.

J Child Psychol Psychiatry

51

1377

1385

74. Stevens-WatkinsD

RostoskyS

2010

Binge drinking in African American males from adolescence to young adulthood: the protective influence of religiosity, family connectedness, and close friends' substance use.

Subst Use Misuse

45

1435

1451

75. DubowEF

BoxerP

HuesmannLR

2008

Childhood and adolescent predictors of early and middle adulthood alcohol use and problem drinking: the Columbia County Longitudinal Study.

Addiction

103

36

47

76. LeonDA

McCambridgeJ

2006

Liver cirrhosis mortality rates in Britain from 1950 to 2002: an analysis of routine data.

Lancet

367

52

56

77. LeonDA

McCambridgeJ

2006

Liver cirrhosis mortality rates in Britain, 1950 to 2002.

Lancet

367

645

645

78. ZuckerRA

2008

Anticipating problem alcohol use developmentally from childhood into middle adulthood: what have we learned?

Addiction

103

100

108

79. FoxcroftDR

IrelandD

Lister-SharpDJ

LoweG

BreenR

2003

Longer-term primary prevention for alcohol misuse in young people: a systematic review.

Addiction

98

397

411

80. BaborT

CaetanoR

CasswellS

EdwardsG

GiesbrechtN

2003

No ordinary commodity: alcohol and public policy.

Oxford

Oxford University Press

81. AndersonP

ChisholmD

FuhrDC

2009

Effectiveness and cost-effectiveness of policies and programmes to reduce the harm caused by alcohol.

Lancet

373

2234

2246

82. ToumbourouJW

StockwellT

NeighborsC

MarlattGA

SturgeJ

2007

Interventions to reduce harm associated with adolescent substance use.

Lancet

369

1391

1401

Štítky
Interní lékařství

Článek vyšel v časopise

PLOS Medicine


2011 Číslo 2
Nejčtenější tento týden
Nejčtenější v tomto čísle
Kurzy

Zvyšte si kvalifikaci online z pohodlí domova

Aktuální možnosti diagnostiky a léčby litiáz
nový kurz
Autoři: MUDr. Tomáš Ürge, PhD.

Střevní příprava před kolonoskopií
Autoři: MUDr. Klára Kmochová, Ph.D.

Závislosti moderní doby – digitální závislosti a hypnotika
Autoři: MUDr. Vladimír Kmoch

Aktuální možnosti diagnostiky a léčby AML a MDS nízkého rizika
Autoři: MUDr. Natália Podstavková

Jak diagnostikovat a efektivně léčit CHOPN v roce 2024
Autoři: doc. MUDr. Vladimír Koblížek, Ph.D.

Všechny kurzy
Přihlášení
Zapomenuté heslo

Zadejte e-mailovou adresu, se kterou jste vytvářel(a) účet, budou Vám na ni zaslány informace k nastavení nového hesla.

Přihlášení

Nemáte účet?  Registrujte se

#ADS_BOTTOM_SCRIPTS#