#PAGE_PARAMS# #ADS_HEAD_SCRIPTS# #MICRODATA#

INX-18 and INX-19 play distinct roles in electrical synapses that modulate aversive behavior in Caenorhabditis elegans


Authors: Lisa Voelker aff001;  Bishal Upadhyaya aff001;  Denise M. Ferkey aff003;  Sarah Woldemariam aff004;  Noelle D. L’Etoile aff004;  Ithai Rabinowitch aff001;  Jihong Bai aff001
Authors place of work: Basic Sciences Division, Fred Hutchinson Cancer Research Center, Seattle, WA, United States of America aff001;  Molecular and Cellular Biology Program, University of Washington, Seattle, WA, United States of America aff002;  Department of Biological Sciences, University at Buffalo, The State University of New York, Buffalo, NY, United States of America aff003;  Department of Cell and Tissue Biology, University of California, San Francisco, CA, United States of America aff004;  Department of Medical Neurobiology, Faculty of Medicine Hebrew, University of Jerusalem, Jerusalem, Israel aff005
Published in the journal: INX-18 and INX-19 play distinct roles in electrical synapses that modulate aversive behavior in Caenorhabditis elegans. PLoS Genet 15(10): e32767. doi:10.1371/journal.pgen.1008341
Category: Research Article
doi: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1008341

Summary

In order to respond to changing environments and fluctuations in internal states, animals adjust their behavior through diverse neuromodulatory mechanisms. In this study we show that electrical synapses between the ASH primary quinine-detecting sensory neurons and the neighboring ASK neurons are required for modulating the aversive response to the bitter tastant quinine in C. elegans. Mutant worms that lack the electrical synapse proteins INX-18 and INX-19 become hypersensitive to dilute quinine. Cell-specific rescue experiments indicate that inx-18 operates in ASK while inx-19 is required in both ASK and ASH for proper quinine sensitivity. Imaging analyses find that INX-19 in ASK and ASH localizes to the same regions in the nerve ring, suggesting that both sides of ASK-ASH electrical synapses contain INX-19. While inx-18 and inx-19 mutant animals have a similar behavioral phenotype, several lines of evidence suggest the proteins encoded by these genes play different roles in modulating the aversive quinine response. First, INX-18 and INX-19 localize to different regions of the nerve ring, indicating that they are not present in the same synapses. Second, removing inx-18 disrupts the distribution of INX-19, while removing inx-19 does not alter INX-18 localization. Finally, by using a fluorescent cGMP reporter, we find that INX-18 and INX-19 have distinct roles in establishing cGMP levels in ASK and ASH. Together, these results demonstrate that electrical synapses containing INX-18 and INX-19 facilitate modulation of ASH nociceptive signaling. Our findings support the idea that a network of electrical synapses mediates cGMP exchange between neurons, enabling modulation of sensory responses and behavior.

Keywords:

Caenorhabditis elegans – Animal behavior – Neurons – Synapses – Quinine – Axons – Electrical synapses – cGMP signaling

Introduction

A defining feature of animal behavior is its plasticity. Animals adapt their behavior in order to respond to environmental challenges and physiological changes. Such behavioral plasticity is essential for animal survival and is achieved by changing the activity of neurons and circuits in a variety of ways. One way is through neuromodulation, whereby diffusible signals such as neuropeptides, dopamine, and serotonin are used to tune brain activity in broad regions [13]. By contrast, neuronal activity can be altered locally by changing the strength of individual synapses [4, 5]. In order to understand dynamic brain function, it is crucial to uncover mechanisms that drive neuroplasticity at various levels.

Electrical synapses (also known as gap junctions) are composed of membrane channels that join the cytoplasm of two cells [6]. They are found throughout vertebrate and invertebrate nervous systems [69] where they pass both electrical and chemical signals between connected cells [10]. Electrical synapses have been primarily studied for their ability to synchronize electrical activity between pairs or groups of neurons [1113], but can also pass small molecules such as calcium [14, 15], cAMP [1619], cGMP [17, 20], IP3 [15, 21], and even small miRNA [22, 23]. Interestingly, while electrical synapses share similar function and protein topology in vertebrates and invertebrates [24], genes encoding electrical synapse components are evolutionarily unrelated [6, 10]. As a result, electrical synapses in vertebrates are composed of connexins, while those in invertebrates are composed of innexins (INXs). The separate evolution of electrical synapses suggests the functional necessity of these channels, although their role in neural plasticity and brain function is not fully understood.

Recently, it was discovered that innexin networks play a crucial role in cGMP-dependent sensory modulation in Caenorhabditis elegans [25]. Krzyzanowski and colleagues found that cGMP functions within the sensory neuron ASH to dampen nociceptive sensitivity but is produced in neighboring neurons [26]. They further showed that cGMP-mediated dampening of ASH nociceptive sensitivity requires an innexin-based network [25]. These findings uncover a new strategy of network regulation that may contribute to the modulation of neural activity. ASH is the primary nociceptive neuron pair in C. elegans and responds with increased calcium levels to diverse aversive stimuli including hyperosmolarity, nose touch, heavy metals such as copper, volatile repellents such as octanol and alkaloids such as quinine [2733]. ASH controls movement away from noxious stimuli through synapses on the forward and backward command interneurons [34, 35]. Nociception in ASH is extensively modulated, and reactivity to aversive stimuli such as quinine is regulated by the presence of food and the satiety state of the worm [25, 3640]. Notably, ASH forms electrical synapses with multiple other sensory neurons and a few interneurons [41, 42], suggesting electrical synapses may be crucial in modulating its activity.

We investigated the impact of electrical synapses between ASH and its neighbor ASK on behavioral sensitivity to the bitter tastant quinine. ASK forms multiple electrical synapses with ASH [42] and expresses several innexins [8, 43, 44], making it a candidate for directly modifying ASH activity. Results of this study show that the electrical synapse proteins INX-18 and INX-19 function within ASK and ASH to allow for modulation of the quinine avoidance response. Through imaging, we found that INX-18 and INX-19 localize to known sites of electrical synapses. Our data further suggest that INX-19 plays a principle role in diffusion of cGMP from ASK to ASH. Our study identifies a direct connection between two sensory neurons that modulates neuronal activity and thus regulates behavior in C. elegans.

Results

Innexin-18 and innexin-19 are required for modulation of the quinine response

A recent study suggests that a network of electrical synapses is involved in modulation of the quinine response [25], however the exact composition of those electrical synapses has not been determined. ASH is a multimodal nociceptive neuron that responds to quinine and forms direct electrical synaptic connections with the sensory neuron ASK [41, 42], which is also involved in quinine sensation [32]. To explore whether the electrical synapses between ASK and ASH play a role in modulating quinine sensitivity, we investigated the innexins INX-18 and INX-19 that are expressed in these two sensory neurons [8, 43, 44]. While INX-4 is also expressed in ASH, we did not include it in our analyses as it has already been explored in a previous study [25].

To determine whether INX-18 and/or INX-19 play a role in modulating the behavioral response to quinine, we assayed inx-18(ok2454), inx-19(ky634) and inx-19(tm1896) mutant animals (Fig 1A and 1B) for quinine sensitivity. We placed drops of quinine solution in front of freely crawling worms and recorded their responses as “responding” if they reverse or “non-responding” if they continue forward [32, 45]. We found that these mutant animals were hypersensitive to 1 mM quinine in the quinine drop test (Fig 1C). As a negative control, we examined the response of mutant animals to M13 buffer. Both inx-18(ok2454) and inx-19(tm1896) animals responded to M13 buffer at similar levels to wild-type (N2) animals, inx-19(ky634) animals, however, were slightly more responsive than wild-type animals (S1A Fig). This may be because this strain has mildly increased spontaneous reversal rates (see below). As a positive control, we tested the response of mutant animals to a high concentration of quinine (10 mM) that that is strongly aversive to wild-type animals. We found that that all strains respond similarly to presentation of 10 mM quinine (S1B Fig). Together, these data show that inx-18(ok2454), inx-19(ky634) and inx-19(tm1896)mutant animals have increased quinine avoidance, suggesting that ASH activity is increased in the absence of these electrical synapse components.

Fig. 1. Mutations in inx-19 and inx-18 result in hypersensitivity to quinine.
Mutations in <i>inx-19</i> and <i>inx-18</i> result in hypersensitivity to quinine.
A,B) Diagram of inx-19 and inx-18 alleles used. Innexin genes code for proteins that consist of 4 transmembrane helices with intracellular N and C tails. Inx-19(ky634) is a SNP resulting in an E>K substitution within the first extracellular loop, while inx-19(tm1896) is an in-frame deletion of 546bp that removes most of the intracellular loop and a portion of the third transmembrane domain. Inx-18(ok2454) is a ~1800bp deletion that removes the second-fourth transmembrane domains and a portion of the C-terminus. C) Quinine Drop Test with 1 mM quinine. Inx-19(ky634), inx-19(tm1896), and inx-18(ok2454) mutant animals are hypersensitive to 1 mM quinine, responding a greater percentage of the time. N2 (wild-type) = 18%, n = 510; inx-19(ky634) = 65%, n = 120, p<0.0001; inx-19(tm1896) = 44%, n = 390, p<0.0001; inx-18(ok2454) = 44%, n = 350, p<0.0001.

The inx-19(tm1896) allele alters quinine responses without affecting locomotion

Two different inx-19 alleles (tm1896 and ky634) have been identified and implicated in sensory neuron function [43]. While mutant animals with either allele show increased response to 1 mM quinine (Fig 1C), these two alleles have different impacts on locomotion. First, inx-19(ky634) mutant animals exhibited more reversals in response to M13 (S1A Fig). Second, during locomotion, inx-19(ky634) animals spontaneously reversed more frequently in the absence of stimuli (S2A Fig). Third, the average crawling velocity of inx-19(ky634) mutant animals was lower than that of wild-type animals (S2B Fig). These data suggest that inx-19(ky634) animals have altered movement in addition to changes in quinine response. At a molecular level, inx-19(ky634) is a G→A single nucleotide polymorphism causing an E70K substitution within the first extracellular loop of INX-19, while inx-19(tm1896) is a 546 basepair deletion that removes the majority of the first intracellular loop and a portion of the second transmembrane domain of INX-19 (Fig 1A). Because the function of innexins requires their transmembrane domains, tm1896 is likely to be a strong loss-of-function or null allele. By contrast, a substitution within the extracellular docking domain may have a more complicated effect on protein function. For this reason, inx-19(tm1896) animals were utilized for the remainder of the experiments.

Inx-19 is required in both ASK and ASH for modulation of the quinine response

Inx-19 is expressed in multiple tissues such as neurons and muscles. Even within the nervous system, inx-19 is expressed in ASH as well as a number of other neurons, including ASK, which has been implicated in quinine sensation and its regulation [32, 43, 44]. To determine the site of action of INX-19, we performed a series of rescue experiments with inx-19 cDNA fused to fluorphores in the inx-19(tm1896) background. We found that, under the control of the native inx-19 promoter [43], expression of inx-19 cDNA fully rescued quinine hypersensitivity in response to 1 mM quinine (Fig 2A). This demonstrates that inx-19 cDNA is functional and the inx-19 mutation is responsible for the quinine hypersensitivity phenotype. Interestingly, these worms also showed reduced response to 10 mM quinine, suggesting that INX-19 overexpression could cause over-correction of the quinine sensitivity defects (S3A Fig).

Fig. 2. Expression of inx-19 and inx-18 in ASK and ASH restores wild-type quinine sensitivity.
Expression of <i>inx-19</i> and <i>inx-18</i> in ASK and ASH restores wild-type quinine sensitivity.
A) Expression of inx-19 isoform A cDNA under the native promoter in inx-19(tm1896) animals rescued quinine sensitivity to N2 (wild-type) levels. Expression in ASK (Psra-9, which expresses solely in ASK [46]) or ASH (Posm-10, which also expresses in the tail neurons PHA and PHB as well as weakly in ASI [47, 48]) alone did not significantly rescue the behavior, while simultaneous expression did. N2 = 15%, n = 220; inx-19(tm1896) = 46%, n = 210; inx-19;Pinx-19::inx-19cDNA = 18%, n = 100, p = 0.62 vs N2, p<0.0001 vs inx-19; inx-19;Psra-9::inx-19cDNA = 32%, n = 100, p = 0.0009 vs N2, p = 0.02 vs inx-19; inx-19;Posm-10::inx-19cDNA = 37%, n = 110, p<0.0001 vs N2, p = 0.13 vs inx-19; inx-19;Psra-9::inx-19cDNA; Posm-10::inx-19cDNA = 22%, n = 110, p = 0.16 vs N2, p<0.0001 vs inx-19. B) Expression of inx-18 gDNA in inx-18(ok2454) animals rescued the quinine hypersensitivity phenotype, as did expression of inx-18 cDNA in ASK (Psra-9). N2 = 13%, n = 120; inx-18(ok2454) = 48%, n = 120; inx-18;inx-18gDNA = 12%, n = 100, p = 0.84 vs N2, p<0.0001 vs inx-18; inx-18;Psra-9::inx-18cDNA = 14%, n = 120, p>0.99 vs N2, p<0.0001 vs inx-18.

We then expressed GFP or mCherry-tagged inx-19 cDNA under the control of cell-selective promoters to determine in which neurons INX-19 acts to regulate quinine sensitivity. We found that expression of inx-19 cDNA in either ASK or ASH (using Psra-9 [46] and Posm-10 [47, 48], respectively) did not significantly restore the quinine response to 1 mM quinine in inx-19(tm1896) animals. In contrast, simultaneous expression of inx-19 in both ASK and ASH brought 1 mM quinine response rates back to wild-type levels (Fig 2A). As controls, we tested the response of these animals to M13 buffer and 10 mM quinine and found no change in sensitivity (S3A and S3B Fig). These data indicate that INX-19 is required in both ASK and ASH for appropriate modulation of quinine sensitivity.

Inx-18 is required in ASK for modulation of the quinine response

Inx-18 is expressed in a subset of neurons including ASK [8, 44]. However, unlike inx-19, inx-18 is not expressed in ASH, indicating that its site of action resides outside of ASH. To determine whether the altered quinine response rate of inx-18 mutant animals is due to the lack of INX-18 function, we performed rescue experiments using inx-18. Inx-18 does not have an obvious promoter, as several genes lie directly upstream of its genomic position. However, the second intron has been successfully used to drive its expression [49]. To test whether the inx-18(ok2454) mutation is responsible for the quinine hypersensitivity phenotype, we cloned inx-18 gDNA, which included the intronic regions. Expression of inx-18 gDNA was sufficient to restore responses to 1 mM quinine in inx-18(ok2454) mutant animals to wild-type levels, indicating that loss of inx-18 is the reason for quinine hypersensitivity (Fig 2B). Next, we found that the site of action of inx-18 is in ASK, as expression of inx-18 cDNA fused to GFP using the Psra-9 promoter rescued the quinine hypersensitivity phenotype (Fig 2B). As controls, we tested the response of these animals to M13 buffer and 10 mM quinine and found no change in sensitivity (S3C and S3D Fig) These results show that inx-18 and inx-19 have distinct, but partially overlapping, sites of action. Combined, our data indicate that INX-19 must be present in both ASK and ASH, while INX-18 in ASK alone is sufficient to modulate the quinine response.

ASK INX-19 and ASH INX-19 localize to the same regions in neighboring axons

The C. elegans wiring diagram suggests that the ASK and ASH neurons form electrical synapses with one another in the nerve ring [41, 42], which raises the possibility that INX-18 and INX-19 are components of these electrical synapses. As our behavioral results show that inx-19 functions in both ASK and ASH, we examined the subcellular localization of INX-19 in these two neurons using fluorescence microscopy. We drove expression of GFP-tagged INX-19 in ASK and mCherry-tagged INX-19 in ASH. These fluorophore-tagged INX-19 constructs are functional as they can restore quinine responses in inx-19(tm1896) mutant animals (Fig 2A). If INX-19 is a component of electrical synapses between ASK and ASH, we reasoned that INX-19 expressed in ASK would localize to the same regions of the nerve ring as INX-19 expressed in ASH. Our imaging data in wild type animals show that INX-19 forms punctate structures along the axons in the nerve ring when expressed in both cells. As expected, most ASK INX-19 and ASH INX-19 is localized to overlapping puncta, despite the fact that these innexin proteins are in two distinct neurons (Fig 3A–3D). Quantification of these images show that INX-19 expressed in ASK and ASH produces puncta that colocalize 67% of the time (Fig 3H). These data indicate that INX-19 is present on both sides of the ASK-ASH electrical synapses.

Fig. 3. INX-19 and INX-18 colocalize in the nerve ring when expressed in ASK and ASH.
INX-19 and INX-18 colocalize in the nerve ring when expressed in ASK and ASH.
A) Diagram of the C. elegans head in a dorsal view. Dashed box indicates the location of imaging of ASK and ASH axons in the nerve ring. B-D) INX-19 expressed in both ASK (where it is tagged with GFP) (B) and ASH (where it is tagged with mCherry) (C) forms multiple puncta that colocalize along the ASK-ASH axons in wild-type animals. Points of colocalization are indicated with white arrowheads. ASK and ASH are additionally expressing cytosolic mTagBFP2, seen in the axons that traverse the image, highlighted in D. E-G) INX-19 tagged with mCherry expressed in ASH (E) colocalizes in the nerve ring with GFP-tagged INX-18 expressed in ASK in wild-type animals (F). A white arrowhead indicates a point of colocalization. Cytosolic BFP fills the ASK-ASH axons, highlighted in G. H) Quantification of colocalization. In worms expressing INX-19 in ASK and ASH, 67% of nerve ring puncta colocalize (n = 144 puncta in 14 animals). In worms expressing INX-18 in ASK and INX-19 in ASH, ~4% of nerve ring puncta colocalize (n = 81 puncta in 10 animals). Each dot represents an individual worm, and error bars are ±SEM.

INX-18 rarely colocalizes with INX-19

Our behavioral results indicate that INX-18 functions within ASK to modulate the behavioral response to quinine. To investigate where INX-18 resides in ASK, and whether it is functioning in the same synapses as INX-19, we expressed GFP-tagged INX-18 and asked whether it colocalizes with INX-19 in wild-type animals (Fig3E–3G). We found that, like INX-19, GFP-tagged INX-18 forms puncta along the axons (Fig 3F). However, INX-18 showed low levels of colocalization with mCherry-tagged INX-19 expressed in ASH (~4% colocalization, Fig 3H), demonstrating that the vast majority of INX-18 is not in the same synapses as INX-19 in adult animals.

INX-19 localization in ASK requires both inx-18 and inx-19

To determine the relationship between INX-18 and INX-19 localization, we investigated whether the expression patterns of INX-18 and INX-19 are influenced by one another. We expressed fluorescently-tagged inx-18 and inx-19 cDNA in ASK and ASH individually and examined their expression patterns in mutant backgrounds. We found that the number of INX-19 puncta in the ASK axon was significantly reduced in inx-18 mutant animals (Fig 4A). In addition, localization of INX-19 within ASK requires INX-19 in other neurons, as the number of ASK INX-19 puncta was diminished in inx-19(tm1896) mutant animals (Fig 4A). In no cases were the puncta fully eliminated, indicating that only some electrical synapses are affected in each case. We did not observe significant differences in the number of INX-19 puncta in ASH in inx-18(ok2454) or inx-19(tm1896) animals, although the downward trend (Fig 4B) suggests that INX-19 localization in ASH may need both inx-18 and inx-19. In contrast, INX-18 localization does not appear to require INX-19, as the number of INX-18 puncta in the nerve ring remained unchanged in inx-19(tm1896) mutant animals (Fig 4C). This indicates that the localization of INX-18 is independent of INX-19. Taken together, these data suggest that inx-18 plays a role in INX-19 electrical synapse assembly and/or maintenance. Perhaps INX-18 is transiently present in the ASK-ASH synapses during development, but by adulthood INX-18 has been removed from these synapses. Indeed, a number of studies have shown that innexin expression can be developmentally contolled [8, 43, 44].

Fig. 4. inx-18 and inx-19 play distinct roles in ASK-ASH electrical synapse localization and function.
<i>inx-18</i> and <i>inx-19</i> play distinct roles in ASK-ASH electrical synapse localization and function.
A) inx-19 cDNA was expressed using Psra-9 and fluorescent puncta in the nerve ring were counted in N2 (wild-type), inx-18(ok2454) and inx-19(tm1896) backgrounds. Each dot represents an individual worm and error bars are ±SEM. Ordinary one-way ANOVA between three groups showed significant differences (F[2,12] = 5.763, p = 0.02, α = 0.05). Dunnett’s multiple comparison test showed that INX-19 ASK puncta were decreased in inx-18(ok2454) (n = 5, p = 0.01) and in inx-19(tm1896) (n = 5, p = 0.05) in comparison to N2 (n = 5). B) inx-19 cDNA was expressed using Psrd-10 and puncta in the nerve ring were counted in N2, inx-18(ok2454) and inx-19(tm1896) backgrounds. Each dot represents an individual worm and error bars are ±SEM. Ordinary one-way ANOVA between three groups showed no significant differences (F[2,14] = 0.814, p = 0.46, α = 0.05). C) inx-18 cDNA was expressed using Psra-9 and puncta in the nerve ring were counted in N2, inx-18(ok2454) and inx-19(tm1896) backgrounds. Each dot represents an individual worm and error bars are ±SEM. Ordinary one-way ANOVA between three groups showed no significant differences (F[2,13] = 1.637, p = 0.23, α = 0.05). D) Inx-18(ok2454);inx-19(tm1896) double mutant animals were assayed for sensitivity to 1 mM quinine using the quinine drop test. Double mutants responded at higher rates than either inx-18 or inx-19 single mutants. N2 = 18%, n = 510; inx-19(tm1896) = 44%, n = 390; inx-18(ok2454) = 44%, n = 350; inx-19;inx-18 = 53%, n = 180, p = 0.05 vs inx-19, p = 0.05 vs inx-18.

Inx-18 and inx-19 have largely overlapping functions

To investigate the functional relationship between inx-18 and inx-19, we assessed the behavioral responses of inx-18; inx-19 double mutant animals. If these two genes act in parallel to regulate quinine sensitivity, the phenotype of the double mutant should be stronger than that of the single mutants. If, however, inx-18 and inx-19 are acting together in the same pathway, we would expect animals with mutations in both genes to have a phenotype of similar strength to the single mutant animals. The inx-19(tm1896); inx-18(ok254) double mutants responded at somewhat higher rates than both the inx-18(ok2454) and inx-19(tm1896) single mutants (Fig 4D), but this difference was statistically insignificant. This suggests that the two genes function largely in the same pathway to modulate the quinine response. Together with the visualization data, these findings suggest that while INX-18 is localized to different electrical synapses than INX-19, its primary function is to set up or maintain INX-19 localization.

Three different possibilities for the function of the ASK-ASH electrical synapses in quinine regulation

In order to determine how inx-18 and inx-19 affect ASH activity, we considered three potential mechanisms: First, inx-18 and inx-19 mutations may alter the cell fate of ASK or ASH, leading to changes in the quinine sensing circuit. Second, the ASK-ASH electrical synapses could function to shunt calcium, depressing ASH activity by allowing calcium ions to flow out to ASK. In this case, we expect that removal of ASK-ASH electrical synapses would result in increased Ca2+ signals in ASH and decreased Ca2+ levels in ASK. Finally, the ASK-ASH electrical synapses could pass cGMP from ASK to ASH, thus down-regulating the quinine response in ASH. Indeed, it was previously demonstrated that expressing the guanylyl cyclase GCY-27 in ASK rescued the quinine hypersensitivity in gcy-27(ok3653) mutant animals [26], suggesting an important role of cGMP in ASK in modulating quinine responses. We tested these three possibilities by examining cell fate markers, the calcium indicator GCaMP6s, and the fluorescent cGMP reporter FlincG3 in ASK and ASH.

ASK and ASH cell fate and morphology are unchanged in inx-19 and inx-18 mutant animals

Electrical synapse channels are known to regulate cell fate decisions during development [50, 51], in particular, inx-19 has been shown to regulate neural differentiation in C. elegans [43]. Thus, it is possible that inx-19 or inx-18 also impacts ASK and/or ASH cell fate or morphology. To test this possibility, we expressed mCherry in ASK (using the sra-9 promoter) and mTagBFP2 in ASH (using the osm-10 promoter, which also expresses weakly in ASI). We found that the cell fate of ASK and ASH remained the same in the inx-18(ok2454) and inx-19(tm1896) mutant animals, as the number of neurons that expressed these fluorescent markers and their positions were unaltered (Fig 5). Furthermore, we showed that the morphology of ASK and ASH were identical between wild-type and the mutant animals. Specifically, both ASK and ASH have cell bodies near the terminal bulb of the pharynx, while dendrites extend to the nose tip and axons project into the nerve ring. Additionally, the cell bodies, dendrites, and axons remained clearly visible In wild-type, inx-19(tm1896) and inx-18(ok2454) mutant animals (Fig 5B). Together, these data indicate that there is no gross morphological or cell fate changes to either ASK or ASH upon removal of INX-18 and INX-19.

Fig. 5. ASK and ASH architecture is unaltered in inx-18 and inx-19 mutant animals.
ASK and ASH architecture is unaltered in <i>inx-18</i> and <i>inx-19</i> mutant animals.
A) Diagram of neural architecture of ASK, ASH, and ASI in the C. elegans head. The dendrites reach out to the nose while the axons extend from the cell body into the nerve ring around the isthmus of the pharynx. B-D) Representative confocal images of the worm head with Psra-9::mCherry (ASK) and Posm-10::bfp (ASH and weakly in ASI) show cell bodies, dendrites extending to the nose, and axons projecting into the nerve ring. Images on the left include maximum intensity projections of the mCherry and BFP images superimposed upon a brightfield image to show location of cells; images on the right are maximum intensity projections of the mCherry and BFP channels without the brightfield image to show details of the cell architecture. Comparison between N2 (wild-type), inx-19(tm1896), and inx-18(ok2454) (15–20 animals per genotype were imaged) show no major differences in cell architecture.

ASK calcium responses remain unchanged upon removal of ASK-ASH electrical synapses

We examined the possibility that the ASK-ASH electrical synapses function to shunt calcium, thus decreasing behavioral responses to quinine. Previous studies have shown that the ASH neurons respond strongly to quinine with an increase in intracellular calcium [27]. While ASK is known to be a minor player in the quinine response [32], the calcium response of ASK neurons to quinine is unknown. In ASK, attractive stimuli typically result in a decrease in calcium levels, while the aversive stimulus SDS results in a calcium increase [52]. Thus, it is possible that the aversive stimulus quinine also directly triggers a calcium increase in ASK. Alternatively, ASK may receive calcium ions from the primary quinine-sensing neuron ASH via the ASK-ASH electrical synapses. If the ASK-ASH electrical synapses pass calcium from ASH to ASK, this shunting effect would decrease ASH calcium levels in response to quinine as some of the calcium ions in ASH would flow to ASK in wild-type worms. In contrast, in animals lacking the ASK-ASH electrical synapses, we would expect increased calcium levels in ASH as the flow to ASK would be blocked. If ASK receives calcium from ASH, we would expect any quinine-induced calcium signal in ASK to decrease in mutant animals lacking the ASK-ASH electrical synapses.

We expressed GCaMP6s in ASK and ASH to visualize calcium dynamics in those cells in response to quinine presentation. Because both ASK and ASH are involved in blue-light avoidance behavior [53], the GCaMP6s experiments were carried out in a lite-1(ce314) background to eliminate blue-light induced changes of GCaMP6s fluorescence in ASK and ASH. Our results showed that CGaMP6 fluorescence in ASK and ASH increased after switching from buffer to quinine, indicating increased Ca2+ levels in response to quinine (Fig 6A and 6B, blue traces). However, Ca2+ signals in ASH were much more robust than those in ASK, consistent with the role of ASH as the primary quinine-sensing neuron [32].

Fig. 6. ASK Ca2+ responses to quinine presentation are unaltered in inx-18 and inx-19 mutant animals while ASH Ca2+ responses are heightened in both.
ASK Ca<sup>2+</sup> responses to quinine presentation are unaltered in <i>inx-18</i> and <i>inx-19</i> mutant animals while ASH Ca<sup>2+</sup> responses are heightened in both.
A) GCaMP6s fluorescence intensity in ASH in response to 10 mM quinine. Cells were imaged for 30s with presentation of quinine at 10s. The lite-1(ce314) mutation was included to eliminate blue-light induced calcium responses in ASK and ASH. All genotypes showed an increase in ASH GCaMP6s fluorescence in response to quinine presentation, though for lite-1;inx-19(tm1896) and lite-1;inx-18(ok2454) animals the response is larger and faster than that of lite-1(ce314). Averaged GCaMP6s traces are shown and error bars are ±SEM. n = 48 animals for all genotypes tested. B) GCaMP6s fluorescence intensity in ASK in response to 10 mM quinine. ASK showed small increases of GCaMP6s signals and there were no significant differences between genotypes. Averaged GCaMP traces are shown and error bars are ±SEM. n = 24, n = 21 and n = 22 animals imaged for lite-1(ce314), lite-1;inx-19 and lite-1;inx-18, respectively. C, D) Heatmaps showing individual traces from all worms analyzed. Data points in the heatmaps represent GCaMP6s signals normalized to the averaged fluorescence intensity of the first 3 seconds of imaging. E) Quantification of ASH fluorescence change at four seconds after quinine stimulation. One-way ANOVA between three groups showed significant differences (F[2,141] = 3.89, p = 0.02, α = 0.05), and Dunnett’s multiple comparison test showed that mean ASH GCaMP6s fluorescence change in lite-1(ce314) animals (n = 48) differed from both lite-1;inx-19 (n = 48, p = 0.02) and lite-1;inx-18 (n = 48, p = 0.05) animals. F) Quantification of ASK fluorescence change four seconds after quinine stimulation. One-way ANOVA between three groups showed no significant differences in ASK GCaMP6s fluorescence (F[2,64] = 0.202, p = 0.817, α = 0.05) between lite-1(ce314) (n = 24), lite-1;inx-19 (n = 21) and lite-1;inx-18 animals (n = 22).

To examine the impact of electrical synapses on Ca2+ dynamics, we monitored ASK and ASH GCaMP6s fluorescence in mutant inx-18(ok2454) and inx-19(tm1896) animals. We found that the increase in ASK GCaMP6s fluorescence remained the same between wild-type and mutant worms (Fig 6B, 6D and 6F), suggesting that the ASK-ASH electrical synapses are not a main conduit for the ASK Ca2+ signal. When we imaged GCaMP6s fluorescence in ASH, we found the increase in ASH GCaMP6s fluorescence were enhanced in inx-18 (ok2454) and inx-19(tm1896) animals (Fig 6A, 6C and 6E). These results are consistent with the behavioral quinine hypersensitivity observed in these mutant worms. Together, these data show that ASK Ca2+ signals do not rely on the ASK-ASH electrical synapses, indicating that Ca2+ shunting to ASK is not the primary mechanism of quinine response regulation.

cGMP levels in ASK and ASH are influenced by ASK-ASH electrical synapses

cGMP is required within ASH for down regulation of the quinine response [26]. Recently, two studies suggested that guanylyl cyclase expression in other neurons plays a key role in modulating the quinine response [25, 26]. These findings prompted us to examine whether ASH acquires cGMP through the ASK-ASH electrical synapses. Indeed, ASK expresses the guanylyl cyclases ODR-1 and GCY-27 [54], both of which are known to modify the quinine response [25, 26]. If ASK supplies ASH with cGMP through the ASK-ASH electrical synapses, we would expect to observe diminished levels of cGMP in ASH with a compensatory increase within ASK in inx-18(ok2454) and inx-19(tm1896) mutant animals.

To visualize levels of cGMP within ASK and ASH, we utilized the C. elegans codon-optimized version of FlincG3, which contains the cGMP binding domains of protein kinase G1α fused to cpEGFP [55, 56]. Binding of cGMP increases FlincG3 fluorescence. We co-expressed FlincG3 and the red fluorescent protein mScarlet under control of the same promoters in ASK and ASH in the lite-1(ce314) background (Fig 7A). After crossing the transgenes into inx-18(ok2454) and inx-19(tm1896), we imaged FlincG3 fluorescence in ASK and ASH. FlincG3 fluorescence was compared to mScarlet fluorescence to account for variations in expression levels. We found that ASH FlincG3 fluorescence was decreased in both inx-18(ok2454) and inx-19(tm1896) mutant animals (Fig 7B), suggesting a reduction of the basal cGMP levels in ASH. These data are consistent with the behavioral hyper-responsiveness of inx-18 and inx-19 mutant worms to dilute quinine, as decreased cGMP levels could lead to increased ASH calcium levels in response to quinine [25, 26]. In ASK, FlincG3 fluorescence was increased in inx-19(tm1896) mutant animals but was unchanged in inx-18(ok2454) animals (Fig 7C), suggesting that INX-19-based electrical synapses are primarily responsible for supplying ASH with cGMP from ASK. Together, our data suggest that INX-18 and INX-19 are major components of the ASK-ASH electrical synapses that modulate behavioral sensitivity to quinine, and that they do so by affecting transport of cGMP into ASH.

Fig. 7. Mutations in inx-18 and inx-19 disrupt endogenous cGMP levels in ASK and ASH.
Mutations in <i>inx-18</i> and <i>inx-19</i> disrupt endogenous cGMP levels in ASK and ASH.
A) Diagram of FlincG3. The cGMP binding domains of PKG 1α (blue) are followed by circularly permuted EGFP (green) and a short PKG 1α tail (blue). WingG2 increases in brightness in response to cGMP. B) Example of FlincG3 and mScarlet expression within ASH. Ellipses were drawn around the cell body to measure fluorescence intensity. C) cGMP levels within the ASH cell body. The ratio between mean fluorescence intensity of FlincG3 and mScarlet signals was determined for each genotype. Decreases in ASH FlincG3 fluorescence were found in inx-18(ok2454) and inx-19(tm1896) mutant animals when compared to wild-type worms. Each data point was obtained from a single cell; error bars are ±SEM. One-way ANOVA between three groups showed significant differences (F[2,68] = 3.643, p = 0.03, α = 0.05), and Dunnett’s multiple comparison test showed that mean fluorescence intensity in lite-1(ce314) (n = 24) cells differed from both lite-1;inx-18 cells (n = 24, p = 0.05) and lite-1;inx-19 cells (n = 23, p = 0.04). D) cGMP levels within the ASK cell body. ASK FlincG3 fluorescence was not altered in inx-18(ok2454) mutant animals, and increased in inx-19(tm1896) mutant animals when compared to wild-type animals. Each data point was obtained from a single cell; error bars are ±SEM. One-way ANOVA between three groups showed significant differences (F[2,72] = 8.115, p = 0.0007, α = 0.05), and Dunnett’s multiple comparison test showed that mean fluorescence intensity in lite-1(ce314) cells (n = 26) did not differ from lite-1;inx-18 cells (n = 25, p = 0.87) but was increased in lite-1;inx-19 cells (n = 24, p = 0.0008).

Discussion

We showed that electrical synapses between the C. elegans sensory neurons ASK and ASH play an active role in modifying nociceptive behavior via the passage of cGMP between cells. We found that the innexins INX-18 and INX-19 are required within ASK and ASH for proper modulation of the quinine response, as mutant animals lacking these innexins are hyperresponsive to quinine. These innexins form electrical synapses between ASK and ASH, in which INX-19 is a major component, though INX-18 is important for correct localization of INX-19 synapses in ASK. Our study supports a model in which ASK-ASH electrical synapses facilitate the passage of cGMP from ASK to ASH. Within ASH, cGMP downregulates calcium signals in response to quinine stimulation, likely by binding to and activating the cGMP-dependent protein kinase EGL-4 [26], ultimately leading to a reduction neural activity and thus aversive behavior (Fig 8).

Fig. 8. Model of ASK-ASH electrical synapse facilitation of ASH modulation.
Model of ASK-ASH electrical synapse facilitation of ASH modulation.

Our study supports a model in which ASK-ASH electrical synapses facilitate the passage of cGMP from ASK to ASH. Within ASH, cGMP downregulates calcium signals in response to quinine stimulation, leading to a reduction in aversive behavior. INX-19 (orange) is shown on both sides of the ASK-ASH electrical synapses while INX-18 (purple) is shown joining with an unknown innexin and contributing to INX-19-based synapse localization.

Electrical synapses can be made of different combinations of innexin subunits. Homotypic channels contain hemichannels that are composed of the same innexins, while heterotypic channels are made up of hemichannels that are composed of different innexins. The channel composition determines permeability, as heterotypic channels are thought to produce rectified electrical synapses: those that preferentially pass ions and small molecules in one direction rather than equally in both [5759]. Our data suggest that INX-19 is a major component of the ASK-ASH electrical synapses. One possibility is that INX-19 forms homotypic channels. However, some INX-19 synapses do contain INX-18, suggesting that at least some are heterotypic. Though the number of electrical synapses containing both INX-18 and INX-19 is quite small, it is possible that levels of INX-18 within such synapses are generally low, making their visualization difficult. INX-18 could also make electrical synapses with other innexins in ASH. Nonetheless, our results suggest that the main function of INX-18 is carried out through its regulation of INX-19, as the inx-18 and inx-19 mutants do not show additive responses to 1mM quinine.

The structural makeup of the ASK-ASH electrical synapses has functional implications for ASH modulation. The composition of electrical synapses is key in determining their permeability of small molecules (such as Ca2+ and cGMP), and heterotypic composition is a major cause of rectification [57, 5961]. If the ASK-ASH electrical synapses are heterotypic (i.e., consist of both INX-18 and INX-19 hemichannels) and rectified, this could explain why ASK cGMP levels, but not calcium levels, are affected by inx-18 and inx-19 mutations. Rectified channels bias the direction of movement of ions and molecules, making it more likely for signals to travel in one direction. If small molecule signals could easily pass from ASK to ASH but not in the reverse direction, cGMP may be more likely to travel from ASK to ASH than Ca2+ would be from ASH to ASK. This mechanism could explain why our data suggest movement of cGMP but not Ca2+. Additionally, the permeability of electrical synapses is dependent on the subunits that make up the channels [17, 62]. While the permeability of most innexin-based channels is unknown, it is possible that the ASK-ASH electrical synapses are more permeable to cGMP than Ca2+, particularly given the timescales upon which each operate. Electrical synapses have long been considered low-pass filters, preferentially passing signals that change over longer time periods as opposed to quick oscillations [63, 64]. Regardless of the molecular reason, the selectivity of electrical synapses to either particular molecules or directions means that they can be sophisticated players within neural circuits. Changes in innexin composition during development or in mature circuits could dramatically impact how the neurons are regulated through the electrical synaptic network.

Electrical synapses are not static structures; they are regulated developmentally as well as in mature circuits [44, 63, 6567]. Our data suggest that innexins can impact the localization of other innexins even if they are not a permanent part of the same synapses. INX-18 plays a crucial role in the localization of INX-19. Thus, its main impact on modulating the quinine response may be in supporting the function of INX-19. While INX-18 is required for proper localization of INX-19, an inx-18 mutation does not eliminate INX-19 synapses completely. This may explain why the inx-18(ok2454) mutation does not have an impact on cGMP levels in ASK, as some signaling could still occur through the remaining INX-19-based electrical synapses even in the absence of INX-18.

ASH activity is modulated by cGMP, and yet ASH is not known to express any guanylyl cyclases, which produce cGMP [54, 68, 69]. This suggests that other neurons may regulate its activity. Such modulation occurs in the context of a larger sensory neuron network that simultaneously assesses many different sensory inputs, any of which could be affecting baseline levels of cGMP within sensory neurons. Thus, by being sensitive to changes in cGMP levels, ASH is able to receive modulatory information from many neurons simultaneously. ASH receives cGMP from its immediate neighbor ASK as well as other neurons [25], suggesting that cGMP levels within ASH (and thus nociceptive sensitivity) are under the control of a number of external signals. If this is the case, cGMP could be a general signal of the state of the worm, integrating multiple signals to indicate whether it is in a favorable or unfavorable circumstance [7074]. Our data support the notion that electrical synapses regulate function in a sensory neuron network by modulating the passage of small molecules into neurons such as ASH. In this way, multiple sensory inputs such as availability of food or sexual partners, presence of pathogens or other environmental conditions could alter various different behaviors at once.

Materials and methods

C. elegans culture

Strains were maintained at room temperature (20–21°C) on NGM agar plates seeded with OP50 E. coli bacteria. The N2 strain (Bristol, England) was used as wild type. The following mutant strains were used in this study: CX6161 inx-19 (ky634) I, FX01896 inx-19 (tm1896) I, RB1896 inx-18 (ok2454) IV, BJH2183 inx-18 (ok2454) IV;inx-19(tm1896) I, BJH2259 lite-1 (ce314) X, BJH2304 lite-1(ce314);inx-19(tm1896), and BJH2303 lite-1(ce314);inx-18(ok2454).

Transgenes

Transgenic strains for rescue experiments were generated by microinjection [75] of various innexin-containing plasmids (30–40 ng/μl) together with co-injection markers (Table 1). The co-injection markers were Punc-122::gfp (BJP-I15, 20–40 ng/μl) and Punc-122::mcherry (BJP-I14, 30–40 ng/μl). Cytoplasmic fluorophores (mCherry, mTagBFP2, and mScarlet) were injected at 30-40ng/μl. For GCaMP imaging experiments, plasmids (BJP-L136, Psrbc-66::GCaMP6s::SL2::mCherry::let-858utr or BJP-L137, Posm-10::GCaMP6s::SL2::mCherry::let-858utr) were injected at 70 ng/μl into the light-insensitive lite-1(ce314) worms. To quantify cGMP levels, FlincG3 plasmids (pFG270, Psrbc-66::FlincG3::unc-54utr or pFG250, Psrd-10::FlincG3::unc-54utr) were injected at 20 ng/μl into lite-1(ce314) worms.

Tab. 1. DNA constructs.
DNA constructs.

Behavioral assays

Well-fed day 1 adults were used for all analyses. To ensure uniformity of worm age and feeding status, L4 animals were picked onto fresh plates the afternoon before behavior tests. Behavior assays were performed on at least 5 separate days in parallel with controls.

Quinine drop test

The quinine drop test was performed as described previously [31, 32, 45]. Quinine HCl (Sigma-Aldrich Q1125) was dissolved in M13 Buffer pH 7.4 (30 mM Tris-HCl pH 7.0, 100 mM NaCl, 10 mM KCl) to 10 mM. Aliquots were frozen at -20°C. Aliquots were defrosted on the day of the experiment and allowed to reach room temperature before use. Solutions were loaded into glass needles via mouth pipetting through long silicone tubing. Needles were formed from 1.5 mm filamented glass capillaries (World Precision Instruments, Inc.) with a Sutter micropipette puller and the tips opened by breaking with fine forceps. 10cm NGM plates were brought to room temperature on the bench overnight and then left open at room temperature to dry for 2.5–4 hours before being used (plates are appropriately dry when worms leave tracks on the agar that do not immediately disappear). For each assay, 15 worms were placed on a plate and allowed to acclimate for 30 min. Small drops (approximately 1 body length in diameter) of M13, 1 mM quinine, or 10 mM quinine were then delivered via glass needle approximately 1 body length in front of worms. When worms encountered the drop, they were scored as avoiding the drop if they initiated a reversal within 4 s and reversed at least half a body length backwards. To avoid desensitization, worms were not tested with a new solution within 2min of initial drop presentation. The experimenter was blind to the strain when scoring reversals. All rescues except for inx-18 gDNA were performed with C-terminal mCherry- or GFP-tagged INX-19 or INX-18 and expression was verified visually before behavioral experiments. All groups were compared with a Chi-square test and post-hoc Fisher’s Exact tests with Bonferroni’s correction were computed to compare groups

Movement assays

Five worms at a time were placed on 10 cm NGM plates and allowed to acclimate for 1 minute. Video capturing was then carried out using an imaging set up from MBF Bioscience. Freely crawling worms were monitored for 60 seconds at 5 frames per second. Moving velocity at each frame was calculated by the WormLab 4.1 from MBF Bioscience after confirming correct assignment of head location throughout the video. Reversals were denoted with negative values. Comparison of number of reversals/min and mean velocity was calculated using an ordinary one-way ANOVA using Dunnett’s correction for multiple comparisons between all groups. The alpha value was set at 0.05.

Confocal microscopy for imaging synapse and cell architecture

Young adults were paralyzed using 30 mg/ml 2,3-butanedione monoxime (BDM) dissolved in M9. Worms were imaged using an Olympus FV1000 confocal system with a 60x oil objective (NA 1.4). Z-stacks of fluorescent images (0.40 μm step-size for synapses, or 1.20 μm step-size for cell architecture) were taken at the region of interest. Maximum intensity projections of images were obtained using Fiji. For colocalization analysis, mTagBFP2 was cytoplasmically expressed in ASK and ASH in order to visualize axons. The number of INX-18 and INX-19 puncta within mTagBFP2-expressing ASH and ASK axons in the nerve ring was counted. Puncta were scored as colocalizing (containing signal from both channels) or non-colocalizing (containing signal from a single channel). Percentage colocalization was calculated by determining the ratio between the number of colocalizing puncta and the total number of puncta in each maximum intensity projection.

Calcium imaging

GCaMP6s [76] was used for all calcium imaging. Lite-1(ce314) worms were injected with either Psra9::GCaMP6s::SL2::mCherry::let-858utr (ASK) or Posm-10::GCaMP6s::SL2::mCherry::let-858utr (ASH) along with the co-injection marker Punc-122:mCherry. Transgenic lines were crossed with mutant animals to generate lite-1(ce314);inx-19(tm1896) and lite-1(ce314);inx-18(ok2454), which the identical extrachromosomal arrays for imaging. Worms were imaged using a microfluidic olfactory chip [77]. M13 buffer was used to load worms into the chip, and their nose tips were washed with M13 buffer for 30 seconds before each recording. At the start of the recording, animals were exposed to M13 buffer for 10 s before 10 mM Quinine dissolved in M13 was washed in to the chip. The images were captured at 5 frames per second with an exposure time of 100ms on a Leica DMI3000B inverted microscope with a 63x Oil objective and a QImaging OptiMOS camera. The region of interest was defined as a square-shaped area surrounding the desired cell body. Background-subtracted fluorescence intensity values were collected from every sample’s ROI and stored into MATLAB formatted files. Change in fluorescence intensity (ΔF/F%) was calculated by dividing each value by the average intensity of the first 3 seconds of imaging.

cGMP imaging

FlincG3 [55, 56] was used for cGMP imaging. Lite-1(ce314) worms were injected with either Psrbc-66::FlincG3::unc-54utr and Psrbc-66::mScarlet::unc-54utr (ASK) or Psrd-10::FlincG3::unc-54utr and Psrd-10::mScarlet::unc-54utr (ASH) along with the co-injection marker Punc-122:mCherry. Transgenic lines were crossed with mutant animals to generate lite-1(ce314);inx-19(tm1896) and lite-1(ce314);inx-18(ok2454), which carry the identical extrachromosomal arrays for imaging. L4 worms were picked onto fresh OP50-seeded NGM plates 6 hours before imaging to ensure synchronization of age and feeding status. Young adults were paralyzed with 30 mg/ml BDM dissolved in M9. Immobilized worms were imaged using an Olympus FV1000 confocal microscope with a 60x Water objective. Kalman filtering was used to reduce noise. Z-stacks (1.28 μm step-size) were taken through the cell body. Maximum intensity projections were obtained using Fiji [78]. Two elliptical ROIs were drawn in the mScarlet channel: one surrounding the cell body and one capturing background fluorescence from a region near the cell body that did not contain an axon or dendrite. Mean pixel intensity in both the FlincG3 and mScarlet channels was calculated using Fiji and background intensity was subtracted from cell body intensity. The ratio between FlincG3 and mScarlet mean intensity was calculated to control for expression variation.

Statistical analyses

Statistical analyses for all experiments except calcium imaging were carried out as described in the legends for each figure using GraphPad Prism Statistical analysis of the calcium imaging experiments was carried out using a custom written MATLAB program and GraphPad Prism.

Supporting information

S1 Fig [ky634]
and mutant animals respond normally to control solutions.

S2 Fig [ky634]
mutant animals have movement defects.

S3 Fig [tif]
Responses of worms carrying rescue transgenes to negative and positive control solutions.

S1 Table [xlsx]
Archive of raw data for figures.


Zdroje

1. Marder E. Neuromodulation of Neuronal Circuits: Back to the Future. Neuron 2012; 76: 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.010 23040802

2. Lopez HS, Brown AM. Neuromodulation. Curr Opin Neurobiol 1992; 2: 317–322. doi: 10.1016/0959-4388(92)90122-2 1643413

3. Birmingham JT, Tauck DL. Neuromodulation in invertebrate sensory systems: from biophysics to behavior. J Exp Biol 2003; 206: 3541–3546. doi: 10.1242/jeb.00601 12966045

4. Zucker RS, Regehr WG. Short-Term Synaptic Plasticity. Annu Rev Physiol 2002; 64: 355–405. doi: 10.1146/annurev.physiol.64.092501.114547 11826273

5. Citri A, Malenka RC. Synaptic Plasticity: Multiple Forms, Functions, and Mechanisms. Neuropsychopharmacology 2008; 33: 18–41. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301559 17728696

6. Phelan P. Innexins: members of an evolutionarily conserved family of gap-junction proteins. Biochim Biophys Acta BBA—Biomembr 2005; 1711: 225–245.

7. Connors BW, Long MA. Electrical Synapses in the Mammalian Brain. Annu Rev Neurosci 2004; 27: 393–418. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.26.041002.131128 15217338

8. Altun ZF, Chen B, Wang Z-W, et al. High resolution map of Caenorhabditis elegans gap junction proteins. Dev Dyn 2009; 238: 1936–1950. doi: 10.1002/dvdy.22025 19621339

9. Söhl G, Willecke K. Gap junctions and the connexin protein family. Cardiovasc Res 2004; 62: 228–232. doi: 10.1016/j.cardiores.2003.11.013 15094343

10. Hervé J-C, Phelan P, Bruzzone R, et al. Connexins, innexins and pannexins: Bridging the communication gap. Biochim Biophys Acta BBA—Biomembr 2005; 1719: 3–5.

11. Bennett MVL, Aljure E, Nakajima Y, et al. Electrotonic Junctions between Teleost Spinal Neurons: Electrophysiology and Ultrastructure. Science 1963; 141: 262–264. doi: 10.1126/science.141.3577.262 13967485

12. Beblo DA, Veenstra RD. Monovalent Cation Permeation through the Connexin40 Gap Junction Channel. J Gen Physiol 1997; 109: 509–522. doi: 10.1085/jgp.109.4.509 9101408

13. Barr L, Dewey MM, Berger W. PROPAGATION OF ACTION POTENTIALS AND THE STRUCTURE OF THE NEXUS IN CARDIAC MUSCLE. J Gen Physiol 1965; 48: 797–823. doi: 10.1085/jgp.48.5.797 14324989

14. Charles AC, Naus CC, Zhu D, et al. Intercellular calcium signaling via gap junctions in glioma cells. J Cell Biol 1992; 118: 195–201. doi: 10.1083/jcb.118.1.195 1320034

15. Sáez JC, Connor JA, Spray DC, et al. Hepatocyte gap junctions are permeable to the second messenger, inositol 1,4,5-trisphosphate, and to calcium ions. Proc Natl Acad Sci 1989; 86: 2708–2712. doi: 10.1073/pnas.86.8.2708 2784857

16. Murray SA, Fletcher WH. Hormone-induced intercellular signal transfer dissociates cyclic AMP-dependent protein kinase. J Cell Biol 1984; 98: 1710–1719. doi: 10.1083/jcb.98.5.1710 6327720

17. Bevans CG, Kordel M, Rhee SK, et al. Isoform Composition of Connexin Channels Determines Selectivity among Second Messengers and Uncharged Molecules. J Biol Chem 1998; 273: 2808–2816. doi: 10.1074/jbc.273.5.2808 9446589

18. Lawrence TS, Beers WH, Gilula NB. Transmission of hormonal stimulation by cell-to-cell communication. Nature 1978; 272: 501. doi: 10.1038/272501a0 211417

19. Tsien RW, Weingart R. Proceedings: Cyclic AMP: cell-to-cell movement and inotropic effect in ventricular muscle, studied by a cut-end method. J Physiol 1974; 242: 95P–96P. 4376174

20. Shuhaibar LC, Egbert JR, Norris RP, et al. Intercellular signaling via cyclic GMP diffusion through gap junctions restarts meiosis in mouse ovarian follicles. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2015; 112: 5527–5532. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1423598112 25775542

21. Niessen H, Harz H, Bedner P, et al. Selective permeability of different connexin channels to the second messenger inositol 1,4,5-trisphosphate. J Cell Sci 2000; 113: 1365–1372. 10725220

22. Valiunas V, Polosina Y, Miller H, et al. Connexin-specific cell-to-cell transfer of short interfering RNA by gap junctions. J Physiol 2005; 568: 459–468. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2005.090985 16037090

23. Hong X, Sin WC, Harris AL, et al. Gap junctions modulate glioma invasion by direct transfer of microRNA. Oncotarget 2015; 6: 15566–15577. doi: 10.18632/oncotarget.3904 25978028

24. Skerrett IM, Williams JB. A structural and functional comparison of gap junction channels composed of connexins and innexins. Dev Neurobiol 2017; 77: 522–547. doi: 10.1002/dneu.22447 27582044

25. Krzyzanowski MC, Woldemariam S, Wood JF, et al. Aversive Behavior in the Nematode C. elegans Is Modulated by cGMP and a Neuronal Gap Junction Network. PLOS Genet 2016; 12: e1006153. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1006153 27459302

26. Krzyzanowski MC, Brueggemann C, Ezak MJ, et al. The C. elegans cGMP-Dependent Protein Kinase EGL-4 Regulates Nociceptive Behavioral Sensitivity. PLoS Genet 2013; 9: e1003619. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1003619 23874221

27. Hilliard MA, Apicella AJ, Kerr R, et al. In vivo imaging of C. elegans ASH neurons: cellular response and adaptation to chemical repellents. EMBO J 2005; 24: 63–72. doi: 10.1038/sj.emboj.7600493 15577941

28. Kaplan JM, Horvitz HR. A dual mechanosensory and chemosensory neuron in Caenorhabditis elegans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1993; 90: 2227–2231. doi: 10.1073/pnas.90.6.2227 8460126

29. Bargmann CI, Thomas JH, Horvitz HR. Chemosensory cell function in the behavior and development of Caenorhabditis elegans. Cold Spring Harb Symp Quant Biol 1990; 55: 529–538. doi: 10.1101/sqb.1990.055.01.051 2132836

30. Sambongi Y, Nagae T, Liu Y, et al. Sensing of cadmium and copper ions by externally exposed ADL, ASE, and ASH neurons elicits avoidance response in Caenorhabditis elegans. Neuroreport 1999; 10: 753–757. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199903170-00017 10208543

31. Hilliard MA, Bargmann CI, Bazzicalupo P. C. elegans Responds to Chemical Repellents by Integrating Sensory Inputs from the Head and the Tail. Curr Biol 2002; 12: 730–734. doi: 10.1016/s0960-9822(02)00813-8 12007416

32. Hilliard MA, Bergamasco C, Arbucci S, et al. Worms taste bitter: ASH neurons, QUI-1, GPA-3 and ODR-3 mediate quinine avoidance in Caenorhabditis elegans. EMBO J 2004; 23: 1101–1111. doi: 10.1038/sj.emboj.7600107 14988722

33. Bargmann C. Chemosensation in C. elegans. WormBook. Epub ahead of print 2006. doi: 10.1895/wormbook.1.123.1 18050433

34. Piggott BJ, Liu J, Feng Z, et al. The neural circuits and synaptic mechanisms underlying motor initiation in C. elegans. Cell 2011; 147: 922–933. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2011.08.053 22078887

35. White JG, Southgate E, Thomson JN, et al. The Structure of the Nervous System of the Nematode Caenorhabditis elegans. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 1986; 314: 1–340. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1986.0056 22462104

36. Ezcurra M, Tanizawa Y, Swoboda P, et al. Food sensitizes C. elegans avoidance behaviours through acute dopamine signalling. EMBO J 2011; 30: 1110–1122. doi: 10.1038/emboj.2011.22 21304491

37. Ardiel EL, Giles AC, Yu AJ, et al. Dopamine receptor DOP-4 modulates habituation to repetitive photoactivation of a C. elegans polymodal nociceptor. Learn Mem 2016; 23: 495–503. doi: 10.1101/lm.041830.116 27634141

38. Chao MY, Komatsu H, Fukuto HS, et al. Feeding status and serotonin rapidly and reversibly modulate a Caenorhabditis elegans chemosensory circuit. Proc Natl Acad Sci 2004; 101: 15512–15517. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0403369101 15492222

39. Ferkey DM, Hyde R, Haspel G, et al. C. elegans G Protein Regulator RGS-3 Controls Sensitivity to Sensory Stimuli. Neuron 2007; 53: 39–52. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.015 17196529

40. Ezak MJ, Ferkey DM. The C. elegans D2-Like Dopamine Receptor DOP-3 Decreases Behavioral Sensitivity to the Olfactory Stimulus 1-Octanol. PLOS ONE 2010; 5: e9487. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0009487 20209143

41. Cook SJ, Jarrell TA, Brittin CA, et al. Whole-animal connectomes of both Caenorhabditis elegans sexes. Nature 2019; 571: 63. doi: 10.1038/s41586-019-1352-7 31270481

42. The C. elegans Wiring Project. A database for C. elegans neuronal connectivity data, http://wormwiring.org/ (accessed 26 February 2019).

43. Chuang C-F, VanHoven MK, Fetter RD, et al. An Innexin-Dependent Cell Network Establishes Left-Right Neuronal Asymmetry in C. elegans. Cell 2007; 129: 787–799. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2007.02.052 17512411

44. Bhattacharya A, Aghayeva U, Berghoff EG, et al. Plasticity of the Electrical Connectome of C. elegans. Cell 2019; 176: 1174–1189.e16. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2018.12.024 30686580

45. Fukuto HS, Ferkey DM, Apicella AJ, et al. G Protein-Coupled Receptor Kinase Function Is Essential for Chemosensation in C. elegans. Neuron 2004; 42: 581–593. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(04)00252-1 15157420

46. Troemel ER, Chou JH, Dwyer ND, et al. Divergent seven transmembrane receptors are candidate chemosensory receptors in C. elegans. Cell 1995; 83: 207–218. doi: 10.1016/0092-8674(95)90162-0 7585938

47. Rongo C, Whitfield CW, Rodal A, et al. LIN-10 Is a Shared Component of the Polarized Protein Localization Pathways in Neurons and Epithelia. Cell 1998; 94: 751–759. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(00)81734-1 9753322

48. Hart AC, Kass J, Shapiro JE, et al. Distinct Signaling Pathways Mediate Touch and Osmosensory Responses in a Polymodal Sensory Neuron. J Neurosci 1999; 19: 1952–1958. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-06-01952.1999 10066248

49. Oren-Suissa M, Bayer EA, Hobert O. Sex-specific pruning of neuronal synapses in Caenorhabditis elegans. Nature 2016; 533: 206–211. doi: 10.1038/nature17977 27144354

50. Lemcke H, Nittel M-L, Weiss DG, et al. Neuronal differentiation requires a biphasic modulation of gap junctional intercellular communication caused by dynamic changes of connexin43 expression. Eur J Neurosci 2013; 38: 2218–2228. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12219 23607708

51. Sahu A, Ghosh R, Deshpande G, et al. A Gap Junction Protein, Inx2, Modulates Calcium Flux to Specify Border Cell Fate during Drosophila oogenesis. PLoS Genet 2017; 13: e1006542. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1006542 28114410

52. Wakabayashi T, Kimura Y, Ohba Y, et al. In vivo calcium imaging of OFF-responding ASK chemosensory neurons in C. elegans. Biochim Biophys Acta BBA—Gen Subj 2009; 1790: 765–769.

53. Ward A, Liu J, Feng Z, et al. Light-sensitive neurons and channels mediate phototaxis in C. elegans. Nat Neurosci 2008; 11: 916–922. doi: 10.1038/nn.2155 18604203

54. Ortiz CO, Etchberger JF, Posy SL, et al. Searching for Neuronal Left/Right Asymmetry: Genomewide Analysis of Nematode Receptor-Type Guanylyl Cyclases. Genetics 2006; 173: 131–149. doi: 10.1534/genetics.106.055749 16547101

55. Woldemariam S, Nagpal J, Hill T, et al. Using a Robust and Sensitive GFP-Based cGMP Sensor for Real-Time Imaging in Intact Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics 2019; 213: 59–77. doi: 10.1534/genetics.119.302392 31331946

56. Bhargava Y, Hampden-Smith K, Chachlaki K, et al. Improved genetically-encoded, FlincG-type fluorescent biosensors for neural cGMP imaging. Front Mol Neurosci 2013; 6: 26. doi: 10.3389/fnmol.2013.00026 24068983

57. Phelan P, Goulding LA, Tam JLY, et al. Molecular Mechanism of Rectification at Identified Electrical Synapses in the Drosophila Giant Fiber System. Curr Biol 2008; 18: 1955–1960. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.10.067 19084406

58. Liu P, Chen B, Mailler R, et al. Antidromic-rectifying gap junctions amplify chemical transmission at functionally mixed electrical-chemical synapses. Nat Commun 2017; 8: 14818. doi: 10.1038/ncomms14818 28317880

59. Palacios-Prado N, Huetteroth W, Pereda AE. Hemichannel composition and electrical synaptic transmission: molecular diversity and its implications for electrical rectification. Front Cell Neurosci; 8. Epub ahead of print 2014. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2014.00324 25360082

60. Marks WD, Skerrett IM. Role of amino terminus in voltage gating and junctional rectification of Shaking B innexins. J Neurophysiol 2013; 111: 1383–1395. doi: 10.1152/jn.00385.2013 24381032

61. Rash JE, Curti S, Vanderpool KG, et al. Molecular and Functional Asymmetry at a Vertebrate Electrical Synapse. Neuron 2013; 79: 957–969. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.06.037 24012008

62. Veenstra RD. Size and selectivity of gap junction channels formed from different connexins. J Bioenerg Biomembr 1996; 28: 327–337. doi: 10.1007/bf02110109 8844330

63. Curti S, O’Brien J. Characteristics and plasticity of electrical synaptic transmission. BMC Cell Biol; 17. Epub ahead of print 24 May 2016. doi: 10.1186/s12860-016-0091-y 27230893

64. Shimizu K, Stopfer M. Gap junctions. Curr Biol 2013; 23: R1026–R1031. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.10.067 24309273

65. Müller DJ, Hand GM, Engel A, et al. Conformational changes in surface structures of isolated connexin 26 gap junctions. EMBO J 2002; 21: 3598–3607. doi: 10.1093/emboj/cdf365 12110573

66. Noma A, Tsuboi N. Dependence of junctional conductance on proton, calcium and magnesium ions in cardiac paired cells of guinea-pig. J Physiol 1987; 382: 193–211. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1987.sp016363 2442361

67. Musil LS, Le A-CN, VanSlyke JK, et al. Regulation of Connexin Degradation as a Mechanism to Increase Gap Junction Assembly and Function. J Biol Chem 2000; 275: 25207–25215. doi: 10.1074/jbc.275.33.25207 10940315

68. Maruyama IN. Receptor Guanylyl Cyclases in Sensory Processing. Front Endocrinol; 7. Epub ahead of print 11 January 2017. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2016.00173 28123378

69. Yu S, Avery L, Baude E, et al. Guanylyl cyclase expression in specific sensory neurons: A new family of chemosensory receptors. Proc Natl Acad Sci 1997; 94: 3384–3387. doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.7.3384 9096403

70. Fujiwara M, Sengupta P, McIntire SL. Regulation of Body Size and Behavioral State of C. elegans by Sensory Perception and the EGL-4 cGMP-Dependent Protein Kinase. Neuron 2002; 36: 1091–1102. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)01093-0 12495624

71. Raizen DM, Cullison KM, Pack AI, et al. A Novel Gain-of-Function Mutant of the Cyclic GMP-Dependent Protein Kinase egl-4 Affects Multiple Physiological Processes in Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics 2006; 173: 177–187. doi: 10.1534/genetics.106.057380 16547093

72. You Y, Kim J, Raizen DM, et al. Insulin, cGMP, and TGF-β Signals Regulate Food Intake and Quiescence in C. elegans: A Model for Satiety. Cell Metab 2008; 7: 249–257. doi: 10.1016/j.cmet.2008.01.005 18316030

73. Singh K, Chao MY, Somers GA, et al. C. elegans Notch Signaling Regulates Adult Chemosensory Response and Larval Molting Quiescence. Curr Biol 2011; 21: 825–834. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.04.010 21549604

74. Raizen DM, Zimmerman JE, Maycock MH, et al. Lethargus is a Caenorhabditis elegans sleep-like state. Nature 2008; 451: 569–572. doi: 10.1038/nature06535 18185515

75. Evans T. Transformation and microinjection. WormBook. Epub ahead of print 2006. doi: 10.1895/wormbook.1.108.1

76. Chen T-W, Wardill TJ, Sun Y, et al. Ultra-sensitive fluorescent proteins for imaging neuronal activity. Nature 2013; 499: 295–300. doi: 10.1038/nature12354 23868258

77. Chronis N, Zimmer M, Bargmann CI. Microfluidics for in vivo imaging of neuronal and behavioral activity in Caenorhabditis elegans. Nat Methods 2007; 4: 727–731. doi: 10.1038/nmeth1075 17704783

78. Schindelin J, Arganda-Carreras I, Frise E, et al. Fiji: an open-source platform for biological-image analysis. Nat Methods 2012; 9: 676. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2019 22743772

Štítky
Genetika Reprodukční medicína

Článek vyšel v časopise

PLOS Genetics


2019 Číslo 10
Nejčtenější tento týden
Nejčtenější v tomto čísle
Kurzy

Zvyšte si kvalifikaci online z pohodlí domova

Důležitost adherence při depresivním onemocnění
nový kurz
Autoři: MUDr. Eliška Bartečková, Ph.D.

Koncepce osteologické péče pro gynekology a praktické lékaře
Autoři: MUDr. František Šenk

Sekvenční léčba schizofrenie
Autoři: MUDr. Jana Hořínková, Ph.D.

Hypertenze a hypercholesterolémie – synergický efekt léčby
Autoři: prof. MUDr. Hana Rosolová, DrSc.

Multidisciplinární zkušenosti u pacientů s diabetem
Autoři: Prof. MUDr. Martin Haluzík, DrSc., prof. MUDr. Vojtěch Melenovský, CSc., prof. MUDr. Vladimír Tesař, DrSc.

Všechny kurzy
Přihlášení
Zapomenuté heslo

Zadejte e-mailovou adresu, se kterou jste vytvářel(a) účet, budou Vám na ni zaslány informace k nastavení nového hesla.

Přihlášení

Nemáte účet?  Registrujte se

#ADS_BOTTOM_SCRIPTS#