#PAGE_PARAMS# #ADS_HEAD_SCRIPTS# #MICRODATA#

Executive task-based brain function in children with type 1 diabetes: An observational study


Authors: Lara C. Foland-Ross aff001;  Bruce Buckingam aff002;  Nelly Mauras aff003;  Ana Maria Arbelaez aff004;  William V. Tamborlane aff005;  Eva Tsalikian aff006;  Allison Cato aff007;  Gabby Tong aff001;  Kimberly Englert aff003;  Paul K. Mazaika aff001;  Allan L. Reiss aff001
Authors place of work: Center for Interdisciplinary Brain Sciences Research, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, California, United States of America aff001;  Division of Pediatric Endocrinology and Diabetes, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, California, United States of America aff002;  Division of Endocrinology, Diabetes and Metabolism, Nemours Children’s Health System, Jacksonville, Florida, United States of America aff003;  Division of Endocrinology, Washington University, Saint Louis, Missouri, United States of America aff004;  Division of Endocrinology, Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut, United States of America aff005;  Division of Endocrinology, University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa, United States of America aff006;  Division of Neurology, Nemours Children’s Health System, Jacksonville, Florida, United States of America aff007;  Department of Pediatrics, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, California, United States of America aff008;  Department of Radiology, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, California, United States of America aff009
Published in the journal: Executive task-based brain function in children with type 1 diabetes: An observational study. PLoS Med 16(12): e32767. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.1002979
Category: Research Article
doi: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1002979

Summary

Background

Optimal glycemic control is particularly difficult to achieve in children and adolescents with type 1 diabetes (T1D), yet the influence of dysglycemia on the developing brain remains poorly understood.

Methods and findings

Using a large multi-site study framework, we investigated activation patterns using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in 93 children with T1D (mean age 11.5 ± 1.8 years; 45.2% female) and 57 non-diabetic (control) children (mean age 11.8 ± 1.5 years; 50.9% female) as they performed an executive function paradigm, the go/no-go task. Children underwent scanning and cognitive and clinical assessment at 1 of 5 different sites. Group differences in activation occurring during the contrast of “no-go > go” were examined while controlling for age, sex, and scan site. Results indicated that, despite equivalent task performance between the 2 groups, children with T1D exhibited increased activation in executive control regions (e.g., dorsolateral prefrontal and supramarginal gyri; p = 0.010) and reduced suppression of activation in the posterior node of the default mode network (DMN; p = 0.006). Secondary analyses indicated associations between activation patterns and behavior and clinical disease course. Greater hyperactivation in executive control regions in the T1D group was correlated with improved task performance (as indexed by shorter response times to correct “go” trials; r = −0.36, 95% CI −0.53 to −0.16, p < 0.001) and with better parent-reported measures of executive functioning (r values < −0.29, 95% CIs −0.47 to −0.08, p-values < 0.007). Increased deficits in deactivation of the posterior DMN in the T1D group were correlated with an earlier age of T1D onset (r = −0.22, 95% CI −0.41 to −0.02, p = 0.033). Finally, exploratory analyses indicated that among children with T1D (but not control children), more severe impairments in deactivation of the DMN were associated with greater increases in hyperactivation of executive control regions (T1D: r = 0.284, 95% CI 0.08 to 0.46, p = 0.006; control: r = 0.108, 95% CI −0.16 to 0.36, p = 0.423). A limitation to this study involves glycemic effects on brain function; because blood glucose was not clamped prior to or during scanning, future studies are needed to assess the influence of acute versus chronic dysglycemia on our reported findings. In addition, the mechanisms underlying T1D-associated alterations in activation are unknown.

Conclusions

These data indicate that increased recruitment of executive control areas in pediatric T1D may act to offset diabetes-related impairments in the DMN, ultimately facilitating cognitive and behavioral performance levels that are equivalent to that of non-diabetic controls. Future studies that examine whether these patterns change as a function of improved glycemic control are warranted.

Keywords:

Cognitive impairment – Behavior – functional magnetic resonance imaging – Neuroimaging – Glucose – attention – Blood sugar

Introduction

Type 1 diabetes (T1D) is the second most prevalent chronic health condition affecting children under the age of 18 years [1,2]. Despite careful monitoring of insulin dosing, diet, and exercise, most children with T1D have difficulty maintaining optimal levels of blood glucose. The question of whether dysglycemia (i.e., abnormal blood glucose levels, which may be characterized as hypoglycemia and hyperglycemia) has an effect on the developing brain—one of the body’s most metabolically active organs—has attracted the attention of both the diabetes and neuroscience research communities. Because childhood and adolescence are periods of major neurodevelopmental change [3,4], disruptions in glucose metabolism during these sensitive developmental periods may have lasting effects on brain development and associated cognition.

Consistent with this notion, several studies [5,6], including research from our group [713], have observed altered brain structure in children with T1D relative to age- and sex-matched healthy controls. Cross-sectional investigations indicate that these changes originate shortly after diagnosis and last well into adulthood [14,15]. Whether morphological changes in the brain contribute to the mild disruptions in cognitive function that have been documented in this population is unclear. Children and adolescents with T1D, for example, are more likely to perform somewhat worse on tasks that require sustained attention, rapid processing speed, memory, and visuospatial functioning compared to their non-diabetic peers [16,17]. Recent meta-analyses show that executive functions, including processes like working memory, attention, and response inhibition, are particularly affected [18].

Given the structural alterations in the brain that have been documented in youth with T1D, it may be considered surprising that children living with this condition do not experience more significant cognitive impairment. One mechanism that may act to preserve normal neurocognitive function in the face of altered brain structure is a compensatory increase in brain activation. Studies of adults with T1D, for example, consistently find diabetes-related hyperactivation in executive control regions during the performance of cognitively demanding tasks [1921]. A hypothesis of compensatory brain function may also account for observations of increased resting state functional connectivity (RSFC) in adults with T1D [2224]. Interestingly, prior work has demonstrated that individuals with T1D scanned after the onset of microangiopathy show no such increase, suggesting that compensatory elevations in brain function may be a finite phenomenon that is eventually lost with disease progression [22]. Moreover, T1D and healthy control groups across many of these studies do not differ with respect to overall cognitive function. Yet high activation and connectivity estimates are consistently correlated in the T1D group with improvements in executive function. These findings, taken together, suggest increased brain activation in T1D may transiently counteract the negative influences of this condition on brain structure and cognition.

In the current study, we sought to extend these findings through examining—for the first time, to our knowledge—brain function in children with T1D as they engaged in a cognitive challenge. Specifically, we scanned 150 children as they performed the classic executive function/response inhibition paradigm, the go/no-go task [25,26]. Widely used to assess attention and inhibition difficulties in a clinical setting, this task requires that participants respond as quickly and accurately as possible, using a button press, to a high number of “go” stimuli (e.g., when they see any letter except “X”), and suppress a prepotent response on a smaller subset of “no-go” stimuli (e.g., when they see the letter “X”). We hypothesized on the basis of prior work in adults, that children with T1D would perform the task as well as non-diabetic (control) children, but would exhibit anomalous increases in activation of task-relevant brain regions, such as the inferior frontal gyrus, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, and posterior parietal cortex. We additionally examined in secondary analyses whether T1D-related alterations in brain activation shared associations with measures of behavior, cognitive function, dysglycemia, and disease course.

Methods

Participants

The study was approved by the institutional review boards at each participating research center and an National Institutes of Health–designated data and safety monitoring board. All parents or legal guardians provided written informed consent, and participants provided assent, according to local guidelines. Participants between 7 and 14 years of age were recruited from and underwent cognitive, glycemic, and neuroimaging data collection procedures at 1 of 5 US clinical centers participating in the Diabetes Research in Children Network (DirecNet) study group (Nemours Children’s Health System in Jacksonville, Florida; Stanford University in Stanford, California; University of Iowa in Iowa City, Iowa; Washington University in St. Louis, Missouri; and Yale University in New Haven, Connecticut). The Nemours Children’s Health System in Jacksonville served as the clinical coordinating center for the study. The Stanford Center for Interdisciplinary Brain Sciences Research served as the imaging and data coordinating center (IDCC) for the study, and research personnel from the IDCC directed all image processing and analyses. Participants in the T1D group were diagnosed at ≥6 months of age and used insulin for at least 1 month prior to study enrollment. Family reports of a prior history of diabetic ketoacidosis (DKA) were confirmed with medical records. Participants in the non-diabetic control group had glycated hemoglobin (HbA1c) result < 6.0% (<42 mmol/mol), fasting glucose < 110 mg/dl (<6.1 mmol/l), and no history of abnormal blood glucose. Sibling controls of participants with T1D (N = 8) also had negative islet cell autoantibody testing within 1 year of enrollment. Exclusion criteria for both the T1D and control groups included past medical history of disorders that could impair neurological development, intellectual disability or significant learning disabilities, psychiatric treatment, premature birth (<34 weeks gestation), low birth weight (<2,000 g), and MRI contraindications.

Study data were acquired from a total of 163 children, including 103 individuals with T1D and 60 non-diabetic control participants. Research personnel at the IDCC reviewed MRI scan and behavioral data for quality. Three scans from the control group and 10 scans from the T1D group were excluded due to poor scan quality because of motion artifacts or because the participant was not performing the task as instructed (see below for further detail on exclusionary criteria). This resulted in final group sizes of 93 individuals with T1D (mean age ± SD, 11.5 ± 1.8 years; 45.2% female) and 57 control participants (mean age ± SD, 11.8 ± 1.5 years; 50.9% female).

Cognitive testing and blood glucose measurement

Cognitive and behavioral assessment was performed with the goal of completing these measures within 4 weeks of scanning. Parents completed the Behavior Assessment System for Children, Second Edition (BASC-2) [27], and the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) [28]. Children were administered portions of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Fourth Edition (WISC-IV) [29]. The order of the testing (with respect to scanning) was not mandated. Age-normed T scores on BASC-2 and BRIEF subscales relevant to the construct of response inhibition (i.e., Externalizing Problems, Hyperactivity, Attention Problems, and Behavioral Symptoms Index in BASC-2 and Inhibition Problems and Global Executive Composite in BRIEF), as well as T scores on the Processing Speed Index in the WISC-IV, were used in analyses examining associations between cognitive ability and brain activation measures.

A hyperglycemic index was determined for participants with T1D based on all available HbA1c values since diagnosis up to the time of participation in this study. As in previous studies [9,10], this index was obtained by computing the area under the curve HbA1c > 6.0% (HbA1cAUC6%) according to the trapezoid rule. Other covariates of interest were computed from data acquired using a continuous glucose monitor (CGM) device that was worn with the goal of obtaining >72 hours of glycemic data (including at least 24 hours of overnight data) across 2 separate 6-day periods within 90 days of the scan. Resulting measures included mean blood glucose; 2 measures of glycemic variability, the coefficient of variation (CV) of CGM glucose and mean amplitude of glycemic excursion (MAGE); and 2 measures reflecting percentage and severity of blood glucose values in hyperglycemic and hypoglycemic ranges, area under the curve above blood glucose 180 mg/dl (10 mmol/l) (AUC180) and area over the curve below blood glucose 70 mg/dl (3.9 mmol/l) (AOCBelow70), respectively. The Jaeb Center for Health Research analyzed all glucose data and provided glycemic metrics.

MRI acquisition

All participants were prepared for unsedated MRI scans through scan simulations performed by research staff at each of the study sites, as previously described [8]. A fingerstick blood glucose level was obtained for participants with T1D immediately before and after each scan session to ensure the glucose level was between 70 and 300 mg/dl (3.9 and 16.7 mmol/l). These 2 measurements were averaged together to compute an average blood glucose level during the time of scan. Rate of glucose change (per minute) during the MRI scan was also calculated, by dividing the change in pre- and post-scan blood glucose levels by the duration of time between the 2 fingerstick tests.

Participants were scanned at 1 of 5 imaging sites using identical Siemens 3T Tim Trio whole body MR systems with matching 12-channel head coils and imaging protocols. T1-weighted structural images of the brain were acquired sagittally, right to left, using a magnetization-prepared rapid gradient echo (MP-RAGE) sequence, with slice thickness = 1 mm, repetition time (TR) = 2,300 ms, echo time (TE) = 2.98 ms, inversion time (TI) = 900 ms, flip angle = 9°, FOV = 25.6 cm × 24 cm, 160 slices, matrix = 256 × 256, voxel size = 1 × 1 × 1 mm, and scan duration 4:54 minutes. Scans were repeated if needed, as per protocol, in order to increase the probability of obtaining a low-motion, usable scan [30]. T2*-weighted functional images of the brain were acquired during the performance of each of 2 go/no-go task runs using an echo planar imaging (EPI) sequence with TR = 2,000 ms, TE = 27 ms, flip angle = 80°, FOV = 22 cm × 22 cm, 33 slices, matrix = 74 × 74, voxel size = 3 × 3 × 4 mm, and scan duration 8:26 minutes.

Go/no-go task design

Stimuli were presented using E-Prime, version 2 (Psychology Software Tools), using a video projector that illuminated a rear projection screen located at the end of the magnet. Participants viewed stimuli through an adjustable mirror attached to the head coil, and MRI acquisition was synchronized with the paradigm. Participants were presented with a fixed series of letters in 80-point font on the center of the screen and instructed to respond, using a response box held in their dominant hand, to every letter (go trial) with a key press, except to the letter “X” (no-go trial). Each letter trial was presented for 250 ms and was separated from the subsequent trial with a jittered intertrial interval that ranged from 750 ms to 8,750 ms, during which participants passively viewed a fixation cross. The task was weighted towards go stimuli (N = 300 trials) to build up a prepotent tendency to respond, thereby increasing the inhibitory effort necessary to successfully withhold responding to no-go stimuli (N = 75 trials). The task was divided into 2 separate runs, each lasting 8.3 min. Accuracy of responses and response times (RTs) were recorded.

Behavioral data analyses

Statistical analyses were carried out using SPSS (version 22.0). After checking distributions with the Shapiro–Wilks test, and transforming data if necessary, group differences in task performance—including commission errors, omission errors, RT for correct go trials, and the signal detection measure, d-prime—were assessed using multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA), controlling for age and sex. Significance was assessed using a 2-tailed α level of 0.05.

Primary analyses: Group differences in activation

Preprocessing of functional MRI (fMRI) data was conducted in FSL (FMRIB Software Library), version 5.0.8, using FEAT (FMRI Expert Analysis Tool), version 6.0.0. The first 3 volumes of each scan were discarded to allow for stabilization of longitudinal magnetization. The remaining images were preprocessed using a series of steps. First, non-brain material was removed from both the anatomical and functional images using the Brain Extraction Tool [31]. Preprocessing included motion correction to the mean image [32], spatial smoothing using a Gaussian smoothing kernel of 6-mm FWHM, and high-pass temporal filtering [33]. Linear registration was performed using FMRIB’s Linear Image Registration Tool to linearly align each individual’s functional data to his/her high-resolution anatomical image [34]. Nonlinear registration was used to align each individual’s anatomical image to standardized space using a publicly available template created for children aged 7–11 years in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space [35,36]. The linear and nonlinear transformations were combined to register each individual’s functional data to template space.

Because 2 separate task runs were conducted for each participant, time-series statistical analyses were carried out at a single-run intraindividual level using a generalized linear model that modeled each condition and accuracy type (go correct, go incorrect, no-go correct, no-go incorrect) using a synthetic hemodynamic response function and its first derivative, as well as motion correction parameters and time points that exceeded a motion threshold (75th percentile plus 1.5 times the interquartile range) defined by FSL’s motion outliers tool (http://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/FSLMotionOutliers). Both runs were combined in a fixed effects analysis for each participant to provide individual-specific summaries of activation.

To test for voxel-based group differences in activation, individual-specific activation summary maps for the no-go correct minus go correct (“no-go > go”) contrast were computed and carried to higher-level voxel-based analyses using FMRIB’s Local Analysis of Mixed Effects (FLAME) [37] to assess a main effect of group while controlling for age, sex, and scan site. Resulting statistical images were thresholded at Z > 3.1 and a cluster probability of p < 0.01, corrected for whole-brain multiple comparisons using Gaussian random-field theory [38].

In addition to voxel-based analyses, region of interest (ROI)–based analyses were conducted to target regions directly involved in response inhibition. These analyses involved 3 main steps. First, data-driven, unbiased spatial maps of activation of executive control regions were created by running the no-go > go contrast across groups, controlling for age, sex, and scan site. To reduce the bias from the fact that the number of participants in the group with T1D was twice that in the control group, we randomly selected 57 participants from the group with T1D to match the number of participants in the control group without diabetes. The 2 resulting balanced groups did not vary with respect to age, sex, or scan site. Second, using voxel-based analyses with FLAME, we tested for the main effect of condition (no-go > go), controlling for age, sex, and scan site, using a threshold of Z > 3.1 and a cluster probability of p < 0.01, corrected [38]. This resulted in a set of clusters that were subsequently isolated and binarized to create functional ROIs. Lastly, percent signal change values for the no-go > go contrast were extracted from each of the resulting ROIs for each participant from the entire study sample (N = 150), using FSL’s featquery tool. Percent signal change values were analyzed using MANCOVA to test for a main effect of diagnosis while controlling for age, sex, and scan site.

Secondary analyses: Associations between activation and clinical and behavioral measures

Associations between activation and behavioral, cognitive, and clinical measures were examined using correlation analyses. After checking distributions with the Shapiro–Wilks test, associations between normally distributed variables were assessed by Pearson correlation. Data that were not normally distributed were analyzed using Spearman correlation. Activation (adjusted for age, sex, and site) was entered as the dependent variable. Behavioral, cognitive, and clinical measures (adjusted for age, sex, and site where appropriate) were entered as independent variables. To reduce the number of multiple comparisons, 2 summary metrics were computed for use in these analyses: mean percent signal change occurring across all voxels contained within significant clusters resulting from our primary (voxel-based) analyses, and mean percent signal change occurring across voxels contained within all data-driven ROIs. MAGE was additionally adjusted for mean blood glucose to discriminate between the effects of glucose variability and glucose mean. Correction for multiple comparisons across these correlations was conducted using the Hochberg step-up approach [39] using a false discovery rate of 0.1.

Results

Participants

The T1D and control groups did not differ with respect to age (t[148] = 0.996, p = 0.321) or sex (χ2 = 0.305, p = 0.616; Table 1). Eight sibling pairs discordant for T1D were included in the study sample. All findings presented below remained unchanged when unaffected siblings from these 8 pairs were removed from the analyses.

Tab. 1. Clinical characteristics of study participants.
Clinical characteristics of study participants.

Within the T1D group, the mean age at TID diagnosis (also referred to as the age of TID onset) was 4.4 ± 2.1 years. The mean glucose during the scan was 166 ± 51 mg/dl. As expected, HbA1c was normal in the control group (median = 5.2%), and hence significantly higher in the T1D group (median = 8.1%, U = 1.500, p < 0.001). Twelve children with T1D (12.9%) experienced 1 or more episodes of severe hypoglycemia since diagnosis; 29 had (31.2%) experienced 1 or more episodes of DKA.

Go/no-go task performance

Commission errors, omission errors, RT, and d-prime values are presented in Table 1. Task performance did not vary as a function of group (F[1,142] = 0.582, p = 0.676). Age was a significant predictor of performance (F[1,142] = 17.430, p < 0.001). Planned follow up comparisons showed age was a significant predictor of d-prime (F[1,142] = 24.099, p < 0.001, β = 0.194), RT to correct go trials (F[1,142] = 12.293, p = 0.001, β = −10.908), number of omission errors (F[1,142] = 21.649, p < 0.001, β = −6.272), and number of comission errors (F[1,142] = 21.649, p < 0.001, β = −1.790).

Cognitive and blood glucose measurement

Within the T1D group, an average of 277 ± 63 hours of CGM data were collected per participant. CGM measures of dysglycemia are presented in Table 1. No differences in BASC-2, BRIEF, or WISC-IV measures of attention, inhibition, executive function, or processing speed were observed between groups (t values[146] < |1.229|, p-values > 0.186).

Voxel-based fMRI analyses

A significant effect of group for the contrast of no-go > go was observed in a cluster that was centered in the posterior node of the default mode network (DMN) and included parts of the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) and precuneus (PCu; x/y/z peak MNI coordinates = 2, −64, 30, k = 2,432 voxels, p = 0.006; Fig 1A). Decomposition of multifactor effects indicated greater deactivation in the control group relative to the T1D group during no-go trials (F[1,142] = 8.119, p = 0.005). No difference in activation was present between groups during go trials (F[1,142] = 1.200, p = 0.275; Fig 1B; Table 2). Two-tailed 1-sample t tests indicated that percent signal change in the PCC/PCu cluster during no-go > go was significantly less than 0 in both groups (t values < −3.500, p-values < 0.002).

Fig. 1. Voxel-based activation differences between groups in the posterior cingulate cortex/precuneus.
Voxel-based activation differences between groups in the posterior cingulate cortex/precuneus.
(A) Statistical activation map from voxel-based analyses, showing the location of activation differences between groups during the no-go > go contrast in the posterior node of the default mode network. (B) Percent signal change from this cluster indicated greater deactivation in the control (ctl) relative to the type 1 diabetes (T1D) group. Planned pairwise comparisons showed that this finding was driven by deactivation differences occurring during no-go trials. Error bars represent standard deviation. *p < 0.01, **p < 0.001.
Tab. 2. Voxel-based activation differences between groups in the posterior cingulate cortex/precuneus.
Voxel-based activation differences between groups in the posterior cingulate cortex/precuneus.

ROI-based fMRI analyses

Data-driven creation of executive control ROIs, created by running the no-go > go contrast across balanced groups, resulted in the following 8 ROIs: the left and right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex/frontal pole (MNI x/y/z centers of gravity = −33, 42, 23, and 36, 44, 22, respectively; k = 1,516 voxels and 2,394 voxels, respectively), the left and right supramarginal gyri (MNI x/y/z centers of gravity = −58, −46, 26, and 54, −38, 22 respectively; k = 2,918 voxels and 6,290 voxels, respectively), a region encompassing the left anterior insula and left inferior frontal cortex (MNI x/y/z center of gravity = −32, 13, −3; k = 4,316 voxels), a region including the right anterior insula, right inferior frontal cortex, and right dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (MNI x/y/z center of gravity = 21, 11, 18; k = 15,439 voxels), the left primary visual cortex (MNI x/y/z center of gravity = −10, −79, 5; k = 658 voxels), and the left cerebellum (MNI x/y/z center of gravity = −32, −56, −36; k = 1,067 voxels; Fig 2; Table 3). Analyses of percent signal change occurring in these areas during the contrast of no-go > go indicated a significant main effect of group (F[8,132] = 132.000, p = 0.010), indicating that the overall profile of activation across these ROIs was significantly different between the 2 groups (average percent signal change across ROIs: T1D, 0.116% ± 0.131%; control, 0.105% ± 0.104%). However, individual post hoc ROI pairwise comparisons were not significant (F values[1,139] < 3.570, p-values > 0.061).

Fig. 2. Mean activation plotted separately for each group and for each region of interest during the contrast of no-go > go.
Mean activation plotted separately for each group and for each region of interest during the contrast of no-go > go.
See text for details on creation of regions of interest. Error bars represent standard deviation. CTL, control; dACC, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex; DLPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; IFG, inferior frontal gyrus; SMG, supramarginal gyrus; T1D, type 1 diabetes.
Tab. 3. Activation in data-driven regions of interest.
Activation in data-driven regions of interest.

Secondary analyses: Associations between activation and clinical variables

Lower suppression of the PCC/PCu was associated in the T1D group with an earlier age of TID onset (r = −0.22, 95% CI −0.41 to −0.02, p = 0.033). After correction for multiple comparisons, no associations were observed in either the PCC/PCu or executive control regions with HbA1c, lifetime HbA1cAUC6%, MAGE, glucose CV, mean glucose, AUC180, AOCBelow70, mean blood glucose at scan, or history of severe hypoglycemia or DKA.

Secondary analyses: Associations between activation and behavioral variables

Comparisons of standardized scores on the BASC-2, BRIEF, and WISC-IV indicated that cognitive and behavioral function was not significantly different between the 2 groups (Table 1). Better scores on parent-reported measures of executive functioning were associated in the T1D group with increased activation in executive control regions. Specifically, increased activation of the executive control network was correlated with lower externalizing behaviors (r = −0.35, 95% CI −0.52 to −0.15, p = 0.001) and reduced hyperactivity (r = −0.29, 95% CI −0.47 to −0.08, p = 0.006). No associations between activation and cognitive measures on BRIEF or BASC-2 were observed in the control group.

With respect to task performance variables, lower response times on correct go trials were associated with increased activation of the executive control network in the control group (r = −0.38, 95% CI −0.59 to −0.12, p = 0.004) as well as the T1D group (r = −0.36, 95% CI −0.53 to −0.16, p < 0.001). Correlations were not significantly different between the 2 groups (Z = 1.07, p = 0.285). Associations between activation and d-prime were not observed in either group.

Exploratory associations between the PCC/PCu and executive control regions

To better understand the nature of possible associations between suppressed deactivation of the PCC/PCu and increased activation of executive control regions, correlation analyses were conducted between these measures across individuals, separately for each group. This analysis used 2 individual-specific mean percent signal change values: the average change across voxels within the PCC/PCu cluster (identified in between-group voxel-based analyses of the no-go > go contrast), and the average change occurring across voxels contained within all 8 executive control ROIs (during this same contrast). Individual-specific mean percent signal change values were adjusted for age, sex, and scan site and entered into bivariate Pearson correlations.

Results of this exploratory analysis demonstrated a significant correlation between the PCC/PCu and executive control regions in the T1D group (r = 0.28, 95% CI 0.08 to 0.46, p = 0.006; Fig 3); reduced suppression of the PCC/PCu was associated with increased activation of executive control regions. This correlation was not present in the control group (r = 0.11, 95% CI −0.16 to 0.36, p = 0.423). Correlations were not significantly different between the 2 groups (Z = −1.18, p = 0.238).

Fig. 3. Scatterplot showing that reduced deactivation of the posterior cingulate cortex/precuneus node of the default mode network is associated with increased activation of executive control regions in children with type 1 diabetes.
Scatterplot showing that reduced deactivation of the posterior cingulate cortex/precuneus node of the default mode network is associated with increased activation of executive control regions in children with type 1 diabetes.
Activation measures represent percent signal change during no-go > go, adjusted for age, sex, and scan site.

Discussion

In this large, multi-site fMRI study, we assessed activation patterns in young children with T1D as compared to age-matched non-diabetic controls while they performed the go/no-go task. Despite equivalent cognitive and behavioral performance between the 2 groups, children with T1D exhibited increased activation in executive control regions (e.g., the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, inferior frontal gyri, cerebellum, and supramarginal gyri), as well as reduced suppression in the posterior node of the DMN (including the PCC and PCu). Secondary analyses demonstrated significant associations (p-values < 0.034) between activation and both behavior and disease course variables. Moreover, exploratory analyses indicated a significant relation between the DMN and executive control regions. Specifically, worse suppression of the DMN in children with T1D was associated with hyperactivation of task-positive regions underlying attentional and executive control. These data support a neural model whereby compensatory increases in activation of executive control networks may transiently counteract T1D-associated abnormalities in brain structure and/or default mode functioning, in order to facilitate normative cognitive and behavioral function.

Our findings are consistent with fMRI studies of adults with T1D [1921], as well as with a previous investigation of RSFC that sampled from a partially overlapping, though considerably smaller, cohort of children with T1D [24]. In this prior study, T1D-associated increases in connectivity were observed within areas having known structural deficit as well as within the dorsal attention network. The dorsal attention network supports sustained attention to external stimuli, and shares significant spatial overlap with the areas identified in the current study as showing increased activation in the T1D group. Similar to this prior RSFC study, although the 2 groups in the current investigation did not differ with respect to overall cognitive function, the magnitude of activation in executive control regions was associated with better parent-reported measures of executive functioning in the T1D group. The patterns of T1D-associated differences in activation and T1D-specific correlations between activation and behavior in this group add support to a neural model whereby increased activation in executive control regions may facilitate near-normal levels of cognitive and behavioral functioning in T1D.

While unexpected, suppressed deactivation of the posterior DMN may provide a neural explanation for the mild cognitive deficits that have been reported in children with T1D [1618]. The PCC and PCu represent a major hub of the DMN. These areas typically deactivate during the performance of goal-oriented cognitive tasks involving attention to external stimuli, and activate during internal, self-referential processes such as rumination and mind wandering [40]. The robust and highly reproducible pattern of activity in this network has led to the proposal that suppression of the DMN—and thus silencing of goal-irrelevant functions—is needed to attain optimal cognitive functioning. Consistent with this notion, functional neuroimaging studies find that greater deactivation of the DMN is associated with more successful performance on stimulus-driven cognitive tasks [4143], including lower error rates and faster response times [44]. Failure to suppress the DMN is also observed in clinical populations in which cognitive impairment is a key feature [4548]. In line with these findings, an earlier age of T1D onset was associated with impaired deactivation of the posterior DMN, suggesting that mounting pathology in this region could lead to diminished cognitive functioning in T1D over time.

Exploratory analyses conducted to explore a possible relation between activation in task-positive executive control regions and deactivation of the PCC/PCu indicated a significant correlation between these regions in the T1D (but not control) group. Similar patterns of increased task-positive activation and decreased task-negative deactivation have been noted in adults with T1D [1921] and in studies of other clinical populations, such as individuals with mild cognitive impairment [49] and persons at risk for Alzheimer disease [50]. Our study is the first, to our knowledge, to test the relation between these networks in children with T1D. The current findings are important in suggesting that hyperactivation of executive control areas may act to offset cognitive dysfunction in T1D specifically caused by an impairment in deactivation of the DMN. Certainly, future studies that take both neural and cognitive function into consideration would be helpful in confirming this interpretation.

Some limitations of this investigation should be noted. We did not clamp blood glucose during scanning. Hence, further work is needed to independently assess the impact of acute versus chronic dysglycemia on the reported fMRI findings. In addition, whether the activation differences we observed between the T1D and control groups are due to inflammatory processes, subtle microvascular disease, the development of advanced glycation end products, or other factors cannot be ascertained in this observational study. Thus, additional research is needed to better elucidate the mechanisms involved in these findings.

In summary, despite equivalent cognitive and behavioral functioning between groups, young children with T1D exhibited increased activation in executive control regions (e.g., dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, inferior frontal gyri, cerebellum, and supramarginal gyri) during the performance of an attention-demanding task. The magnitude of these increases was significantly correlated with deficits in deactivation of the posterior node of the DMN, suggesting a putative compensatory role of brain function in T1D, whereby higher activation in task-relevant regions act both to offset T1D-related impairments in DMN function and to facilitate behavioral performance levels equivalent to those of their non-diabetic peers. Future studies that examine whether these patterns change as a function of improved glycemic control deserves further study.


Zdroje

1. Traisman HS. Management of juvenile diabetes mellitus.St. Louis: Mosby; 1980.

2. International Diabetes Federation. IDF diabetes atlas. Brussels: International Diabetes Federation; 2013.

3. Giedd JN, Clasen LS, Lenroot R, Greenstein D, Wallace GL, Ordaz S, et al. Puberty-related influences on brain development. Mol Cell Endocrinol. 2006;254:154–62. doi: 10.1016/j.mce.2006.04.016 16765510

4. Luna B, Sweeney JA. Studies of brain and cognitive maturation through childhood and adolescence: a strategy for testing neurodevelopmental hypotheses. Schizophr Bull. 2001;27(3):443. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a006886 11596846

5. Siller AF, Lugar H, Rutlin J, Koller JM, Semenkovich K, White NH, et al. Severity of clinical presentation in youth with type 1 diabetes is associated with differences in brain structure. Pediatr Diabetes. 2017;18(8):686–95. doi: 10.1111/pedi.12420 27488913

6. Perantie DC, Lim A, Wu J, Weaver P, Warren SL, Sadler M, et al. Effects of prior hypoglycemia and hyperglycemia on cognition in children with type 1 diabetes mellitus. Pediatr Diabetes. 2008;9(2):87–95. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5448.2007.00274.x 18208449

7. Aye T, Barnea-Goraly N, Ambler C, Hoang S, Schleifer K, Park Y, et al. White matter structural differences in young children with type 1 diabetes: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Diabetes Care. 2012;35(11):2167–73. doi: 10.2337/dc12-0017 22966090

8. Barnea-Goraly N, Raman M, Mazaika P, Marzelli M, Hershey T, Weinzimer SA, et al. Alterations in white matter structure in young children with type 1 diabetes. Diabetes Care. 2014;37(2):332–40. doi: 10.2337/dc13-1388 24319123

9. Marzelli MJ, Mazaika PK, Barnea-Goraly N, Hershey T, Tsalikian E, Tamborlane W, et al. Neuroanatomical correlates of dysglycemia in young children with type 1 diabetes. Diabetes. 2014;63(1):343–53. doi: 10.2337/db13-0179 24170697

10. Mauras N, Mazaika P, Buckingham B, Weinzimer S, White NH, Tsalikian E, et al. Longitudinal assessment of neuroanatomical and cognitive differences in young children with type 1 diabetes: association with hyperglycemia. Diabetes. 2014;64(5):1770–9. doi: 10.2337/db14-1445 25488901

11. Mazaika PK, Weinzimer SA, Mauras N, Buckingham B, White NH, Tsalikian E, et al. Variations in brain volume and growth in young children with type 1 diabetes. Diabetes. 2016;65(2):476–85. doi: 10.2337/db15-1242 26512024

12. Foland-Ross LC, Reiss AL, Mazaika PK, Mauras N, Weinzimer SA, Aye T, et al. Longitudinal assessment of hippocampus structure in children with type 1 diabetes. Pediatr Diabetes. 2018;19(6):1116–23.

13. Hosseini SH, Mazaika P, Mauras N, Buckingham B, Weinzimer SA, Tsalikian E, et al. Altered integration of structural covariance networks in young children with type 1 diabetes. Hum Brain Mapp. 2016;37(11):4034–46. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23293 27339089

14. Nunley KA, Rosano C, Ryan CM, Jennings JR, Aizenstein HJ, Zgibor JC, et al. Clinically relevant cognitive impairment in middle-aged adults with childhood-onset type 1 diabetes. Diabetes Care. 2015;38(9):1768–76. doi: 10.2337/dc15-0041 26153270

15. Ferguson SC, Blane A, Wardlaw J, Frier BM, Perros P, McCrimmon RJ, et al. Influence of an early-onset age of type 1 diabetes on cerebral structure and cognitive function. Diabetes Care. 2005;28(6):1431–7. doi: 10.2337/diacare.28.6.1431 15920064

16. Gaudieri PA, Chen R, Greer TF, Holmes CS. Cognitive function in children with type 1 diabetes a meta-analysis. Diabetes Care. 2008;31(9):1892–7. doi: 10.2337/dc07-2132 18753668

17. Naguib JM, Kulinskaya E, Lomax CL, Garralda ME. Neuro-cognitive performance in children with type 1 diabetes—a meta-analysis. J Pediatr Psychol. 2008;34(3):271–82. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsn074 18635605

18. Broadley MM, White MJ, Andrew B. A systematic review and meta-analysis of executive function performance in type 1 diabetes mellitus. Psychosom Med. 2017;79(6):684–96. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000460 28207612

19. Gallardo-Moreno GB, González-Garrido AA, Gudayol-Ferré E, Guàrdia-Olmos J. Type 1 diabetes modifies brain activation in young patients while performing visuospatial working memory tasks. J Diabetes Res. 2015;2015:703512. doi: 10.1155/2015/703512 26266268

20. Bolo NR, Musen G, Jacobson AM, Weinger K, McCartney RL, Flores V, et al. Brain activation during working memory is altered in type 1 diabetes during hypoglycemia. Diabetes. 2011;60(12):3256–64. doi: 10.2337/db11-0506 21984582

21. Guàrdia-Olmos J, Gallardo-Moreno GB, Gudayol-Ferré E, Peró-Cebollero M, González-Garrido AA. Effect of verbal task complexity in a working memory paradigm in patients with type 1 diabetes. A fMRI study. PLoS ONE. 2017;12(6):e0178172. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0178172 28582399

22. Van Duinkerken E, Schoonheim MM, Sanz-Arigita EJ, IJzerman RG, Moll AC, Snoek FJ, et al. Resting-state brain networks in type 1 diabetic patients with and without microangiopathy and their relation to cognitive functions and disease variables. Diabetes. 2012;61(7):1814–21. doi: 10.2337/db11-1358 22438575

23. Bolo NR, Musen G, Simonson DC, Nickerson LD, Flores VL, Siracusa T, et al. Functional connectivity of insula, basal ganglia, and prefrontal executive control networks during hypoglycemia in type 1 diabetes. J Neurosci. 2015;35(31):11012–23. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0319-15.2015 26245963

24. Saggar M, Tsalikian E, Mauras N, Mazaika P, White NH, Weinzimer S, et al. Compensatory hyper-connectivity in developing brains of young children with type 1 diabetes. Diabetes. 2016;66(3):754–62. doi: 10.2337/db16-0414 27702833

25. Luce RD. Response times: their role in inferring elementary mental organization. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1986.

26. Donders FC. On the speed of mental processes. Acta Psychol (Amst). 1969;30:412–31.

27. Reynolds CR, Kamphaus RW. Behavior assessment system for children. 2nd edition. Circle Pines (MN): American Guidance Service; 2004.

28. Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Guy SC, Kenworthy L. Behavior rating inventory of executive functions. Lutz (FL): Psychological Assessment Resources; 2000.

29. Wechsler D. Wechsler intelligence scale for children, fourth edition (WISC-IV). San Antonio (TX): Psychological Corporation; 2003.

30. Jack CR, Bernstein MA, Fox NC, Thompson P, Alexander G, Harvey D, et al. The Alzheimer’s disease neuroimaging initiative (ADNI): MRI methods. J Magn Reson Imaging. 2008;27(4):685–91. doi: 10.1002/jmri.21049 18302232

31. Smith SM. Fast robust automated brain extraction. Hum Brain Mapp. 2002;17(3):143–55. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10062 12391568

32. Jenkinson M, Bannister P, Brady M, Smith S. Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. Neuroimage. 2002;17(2):825–41. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(02)91132-8 12377157

33. Woolrich MW, Ripley BD, Brady M, Smith SM. Temporal autocorrelation in univariate linear modeling of FMRI data. Neuroimage. 2001;14(6):1370–86. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0931 11707093

34. Jenkinson M, Smith S. A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images. Med Image Anal. 2001;5(2):143–56. doi: 10.1016/s1361-8415(01)00036-6 11516708

35. Fonov V, Evans AC, Botteron K, Almli CR, McKinstry RC, Collins DL, et al. Unbiased average age-appropriate atlases for pediatric studies. Neuroimage. 2011;54(1):313–27. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.033 20656036

36. Fonov VS, Evans AC, McKinstry RC, Almli C, Collins D. Unbiased nonlinear average age-appropriate brain templates from birth to adulthood. Neuroimage. 2009;47:S102.

37. Woolrich M. Robust group analysis using outlier inference. Neuroimage. 2008;41(2):286–301. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.02.042 18407525

38. Worsley KJ. Statistical analysis of activation images. In: Jezzard P, Matthews PM, Smith SM, editors. Functional MRI: an introduction to methods. New York: Oxford University Press; 2001.

39. Hochberg Y. A sharper Bonferroni procedure for multiple tests of significance. Biometrika. 1988;75(4):800–2.

40. Raichle ME. The brain’s default mode network. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2015;38:433–47. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-014030 25938726

41. Anticevic A, Repovs G, Shulman GL, Barch DM. When less is more: TPJ and default network deactivation during encoding predicts working memory performance. Neuroimage. 2010;49(3):2638–48. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.008 19913622

42. Daselaar S, Prince S, Cabeza R. When less means more: deactivations during encoding that predict subsequent memory. Neuroimage. 2004;23(3):921–7. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.031 15528092

43. Anticevic A, Gancsos M, Murray JD, Repovs G, Driesen NR, Ennis DJ, et al. NMDA receptor function in large-scale anticorrelated neural systems with implications for cognition and schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012;109(41):16720–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1208494109 23012427

44. Hayden BY, Smith DV, Platt ML. Electrophysiological correlates of default-mode processing in macaque posterior cingulate cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106(14):5948–53. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0812035106 19293382

45. Buckner R, Andrews-Hanna J, Schacter D. The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2008;1124:1–38. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.011 18400922

46. Anticevic A, Cole MW, Murray JD, Corlett PR, Wang X-J, Krystal JH. The role of default network deactivation in cognition and disease. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012;16(12):584–92. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.10.008 23142417

47. Grady CL, Protzner AB, Kovacevic N, Strother SC, Afshin-Pour B, Wojtowicz M, et al. A multivariate analysis of age-related differences in default mode and task-positive networks across multiple cognitive domains. Cereb Cortex. 2009;20(6):1432–47. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp207 19789183

48. Lustig C, Snyder AZ, Bhakta M, O’Brien KC, McAvoy M, Raichle ME, et al. Functional deactivations: change with age and dementia of the Alzheimer type. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003;100(24):14504–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2235925100 14608034

49. Dickerson BC, Salat DH, Bates JF, Atiya M, Killiany RJ, Greve DN, et al. Medial temporal lobe function and structure in mild cognitive impairment. Ann Neurol. 2004;56(1):27–35. doi: 10.1002/ana.20163 15236399

50. Pihlajamäki M, Sperling RA. Functional MRI assessment of task-induced deactivation of the default mode network in Alzheimer’s disease and at-risk older individuals. Behav Neurol. 2009;21(1–2):77–91.

Štítky
Interní lékařství

Článek vyšel v časopise

PLOS Medicine


2019 Číslo 12
Nejčtenější tento týden
Nejčtenější v tomto čísle
Kurzy

Zvyšte si kvalifikaci online z pohodlí domova

Důležitost adherence při depresivním onemocnění
nový kurz
Autoři: MUDr. Eliška Bartečková, Ph.D.

Koncepce osteologické péče pro gynekology a praktické lékaře
Autoři: MUDr. František Šenk

Sekvenční léčba schizofrenie
Autoři: MUDr. Jana Hořínková, Ph.D.

Hypertenze a hypercholesterolémie – synergický efekt léčby
Autoři: prof. MUDr. Hana Rosolová, DrSc.

Multidisciplinární zkušenosti u pacientů s diabetem
Autoři: Prof. MUDr. Martin Haluzík, DrSc., prof. MUDr. Vojtěch Melenovský, CSc., prof. MUDr. Vladimír Tesař, DrSc.

Všechny kurzy
Přihlášení
Zapomenuté heslo

Zadejte e-mailovou adresu, se kterou jste vytvářel(a) účet, budou Vám na ni zaslány informace k nastavení nového hesla.

Přihlášení

Nemáte účet?  Registrujte se

#ADS_BOTTOM_SCRIPTS#